Have a thesis expert improve your writing

Check your thesis for plagiarism in 10 minutes, generate your apa citations for free.

  • Knowledge Base
  • Null and Alternative Hypotheses | Definitions & Examples

Null and Alternative Hypotheses | Definitions & Examples

Published on 5 October 2022 by Shaun Turney . Revised on 6 December 2022.

The null and alternative hypotheses are two competing claims that researchers weigh evidence for and against using a statistical test :

  • Null hypothesis (H 0 ): There’s no effect in the population .
  • Alternative hypothesis (H A ): There’s an effect in the population.

The effect is usually the effect of the independent variable on the dependent variable .

Table of contents

Answering your research question with hypotheses, what is a null hypothesis, what is an alternative hypothesis, differences between null and alternative hypotheses, how to write null and alternative hypotheses, frequently asked questions about null and alternative hypotheses.

The null and alternative hypotheses offer competing answers to your research question . When the research question asks “Does the independent variable affect the dependent variable?”, the null hypothesis (H 0 ) answers “No, there’s no effect in the population.” On the other hand, the alternative hypothesis (H A ) answers “Yes, there is an effect in the population.”

The null and alternative are always claims about the population. That’s because the goal of hypothesis testing is to make inferences about a population based on a sample . Often, we infer whether there’s an effect in the population by looking at differences between groups or relationships between variables in the sample.

You can use a statistical test to decide whether the evidence favors the null or alternative hypothesis. Each type of statistical test comes with a specific way of phrasing the null and alternative hypothesis. However, the hypotheses can also be phrased in a general way that applies to any test.

The null hypothesis is the claim that there’s no effect in the population.

If the sample provides enough evidence against the claim that there’s no effect in the population ( p ≤ α), then we can reject the null hypothesis . Otherwise, we fail to reject the null hypothesis.

Although “fail to reject” may sound awkward, it’s the only wording that statisticians accept. Be careful not to say you “prove” or “accept” the null hypothesis.

Null hypotheses often include phrases such as “no effect”, “no difference”, or “no relationship”. When written in mathematical terms, they always include an equality (usually =, but sometimes ≥ or ≤).

Examples of null hypotheses

The table below gives examples of research questions and null hypotheses. There’s always more than one way to answer a research question, but these null hypotheses can help you get started.

*Note that some researchers prefer to always write the null hypothesis in terms of “no effect” and “=”. It would be fine to say that daily meditation has no effect on the incidence of depression and p 1 = p 2 .

The alternative hypothesis (H A ) is the other answer to your research question . It claims that there’s an effect in the population.

Often, your alternative hypothesis is the same as your research hypothesis. In other words, it’s the claim that you expect or hope will be true.

The alternative hypothesis is the complement to the null hypothesis. Null and alternative hypotheses are exhaustive, meaning that together they cover every possible outcome. They are also mutually exclusive, meaning that only one can be true at a time.

Alternative hypotheses often include phrases such as “an effect”, “a difference”, or “a relationship”. When alternative hypotheses are written in mathematical terms, they always include an inequality (usually ≠, but sometimes > or <). As with null hypotheses, there are many acceptable ways to phrase an alternative hypothesis.

Examples of alternative hypotheses

The table below gives examples of research questions and alternative hypotheses to help you get started with formulating your own.

Null and alternative hypotheses are similar in some ways:

  • They’re both answers to the research question
  • They both make claims about the population
  • They’re both evaluated by statistical tests.

However, there are important differences between the two types of hypotheses, summarized in the following table.

To help you write your hypotheses, you can use the template sentences below. If you know which statistical test you’re going to use, you can use the test-specific template sentences. Otherwise, you can use the general template sentences.

The only thing you need to know to use these general template sentences are your dependent and independent variables. To write your research question, null hypothesis, and alternative hypothesis, fill in the following sentences with your variables:

Does independent variable affect dependent variable ?

  • Null hypothesis (H 0 ): Independent variable does not affect dependent variable .
  • Alternative hypothesis (H A ): Independent variable affects dependent variable .

Test-specific

Once you know the statistical test you’ll be using, you can write your hypotheses in a more precise and mathematical way specific to the test you chose. The table below provides template sentences for common statistical tests.

Note: The template sentences above assume that you’re performing one-tailed tests . One-tailed tests are appropriate for most studies.

The null hypothesis is often abbreviated as H 0 . When the null hypothesis is written using mathematical symbols, it always includes an equality symbol (usually =, but sometimes ≥ or ≤).

The alternative hypothesis is often abbreviated as H a or H 1 . When the alternative hypothesis is written using mathematical symbols, it always includes an inequality symbol (usually ≠, but sometimes < or >).

A research hypothesis is your proposed answer to your research question. The research hypothesis usually includes an explanation (‘ x affects y because …’).

A statistical hypothesis, on the other hand, is a mathematical statement about a population parameter. Statistical hypotheses always come in pairs: the null and alternative hypotheses. In a well-designed study , the statistical hypotheses correspond logically to the research hypothesis.

Cite this Scribbr article

If you want to cite this source, you can copy and paste the citation or click the ‘Cite this Scribbr article’ button to automatically add the citation to our free Reference Generator.

Turney, S. (2022, December 06). Null and Alternative Hypotheses | Definitions & Examples. Scribbr. Retrieved 22 April 2024, from https://www.scribbr.co.uk/stats/null-and-alternative-hypothesis/

Is this article helpful?

Shaun Turney

Shaun Turney

Other students also liked, levels of measurement: nominal, ordinal, interval, ratio, the standard normal distribution | calculator, examples & uses, types of variables in research | definitions & examples.

9.1 Null and Alternative Hypotheses

The actual test begins by considering two hypotheses . They are called the null hypothesis and the alternative hypothesis . These hypotheses contain opposing viewpoints.

H 0 , the — null hypothesis: a statement of no difference between sample means or proportions or no difference between a sample mean or proportion and a population mean or proportion. In other words, the difference equals 0.

H a —, the alternative hypothesis: a claim about the population that is contradictory to H 0 and what we conclude when we reject H 0 .

Since the null and alternative hypotheses are contradictory, you must examine evidence to decide if you have enough evidence to reject the null hypothesis or not. The evidence is in the form of sample data.

After you have determined which hypothesis the sample supports, you make a decision. There are two options for a decision. They are reject H 0 if the sample information favors the alternative hypothesis or do not reject H 0 or decline to reject H 0 if the sample information is insufficient to reject the null hypothesis.

Mathematical Symbols Used in H 0 and H a :

H 0 always has a symbol with an equal in it. H a never has a symbol with an equal in it. The choice of symbol depends on the wording of the hypothesis test. However, be aware that many researchers use = in the null hypothesis, even with > or < as the symbol in the alternative hypothesis. This practice is acceptable because we only make the decision to reject or not reject the null hypothesis.

Example 9.1

H 0 : No more than 30 percent of the registered voters in Santa Clara County voted in the primary election. p ≤ 30 H a : More than 30 percent of the registered voters in Santa Clara County voted in the primary election. p > 30

A medical trial is conducted to test whether or not a new medicine reduces cholesterol by 25 percent. State the null and alternative hypotheses.

Example 9.2

We want to test whether the mean GPA of students in American colleges is different from 2.0 (out of 4.0). The null and alternative hypotheses are the following: H 0 : μ = 2.0 H a : μ ≠ 2.0

We want to test whether the mean height of eighth graders is 66 inches. State the null and alternative hypotheses. Fill in the correct symbol (=, ≠, ≥, <, ≤, >) for the null and alternative hypotheses.

  • H 0 : μ __ 66
  • H a : μ __ 66

Example 9.3

We want to test if college students take fewer than five years to graduate from college, on the average. The null and alternative hypotheses are the following: H 0 : μ ≥ 5 H a : μ < 5

We want to test if it takes fewer than 45 minutes to teach a lesson plan. State the null and alternative hypotheses. Fill in the correct symbol ( =, ≠, ≥, <, ≤, >) for the null and alternative hypotheses.

  • H 0 : μ __ 45
  • H a : μ __ 45

Example 9.4

An article on school standards stated that about half of all students in France, Germany, and Israel take advanced placement exams and a third of the students pass. The same article stated that 6.6 percent of U.S. students take advanced placement exams and 4.4 percent pass. Test if the percentage of U.S. students who take advanced placement exams is more than 6.6 percent. State the null and alternative hypotheses. H 0 : p ≤ 0.066 H a : p > 0.066

On a state driver’s test, about 40 percent pass the test on the first try. We want to test if more than 40 percent pass on the first try. Fill in the correct symbol (=, ≠, ≥, <, ≤, >) for the null and alternative hypotheses.

  • H 0 : p __ 0.40
  • H a : p __ 0.40

Collaborative Exercise

Bring to class a newspaper, some news magazines, and some internet articles. In groups, find articles from which your group can write null and alternative hypotheses. Discuss your hypotheses with the rest of the class.

As an Amazon Associate we earn from qualifying purchases.

This book may not be used in the training of large language models or otherwise be ingested into large language models or generative AI offerings without OpenStax's permission.

Want to cite, share, or modify this book? This book uses the Creative Commons Attribution License and you must attribute Texas Education Agency (TEA). The original material is available at: https://www.texasgateway.org/book/tea-statistics . Changes were made to the original material, including updates to art, structure, and other content updates.

Access for free at https://openstax.org/books/statistics/pages/1-introduction
  • Authors: Barbara Illowsky, Susan Dean
  • Publisher/website: OpenStax
  • Book title: Statistics
  • Publication date: Mar 27, 2020
  • Location: Houston, Texas
  • Book URL: https://openstax.org/books/statistics/pages/1-introduction
  • Section URL: https://openstax.org/books/statistics/pages/9-1-null-and-alternative-hypotheses

© Jan 23, 2024 Texas Education Agency (TEA). The OpenStax name, OpenStax logo, OpenStax book covers, OpenStax CNX name, and OpenStax CNX logo are not subject to the Creative Commons license and may not be reproduced without the prior and express written consent of Rice University.

Academic Success Center

Statistics Resources

  • Excel - Tutorials
  • Basic Probability Rules
  • Single Event Probability
  • Complement Rule
  • Intersections & Unions
  • Compound Events
  • Levels of Measurement
  • Independent and Dependent Variables
  • Entering Data
  • Central Tendency
  • Data and Tests
  • Displaying Data
  • Discussing Statistics In-text
  • SEM and Confidence Intervals
  • Two-Way Frequency Tables
  • Empirical Rule
  • Finding Probability
  • Accessing SPSS
  • Chart and Graphs
  • Frequency Table and Distribution
  • Descriptive Statistics
  • Converting Raw Scores to Z-Scores
  • Converting Z-scores to t-scores
  • Split File/Split Output
  • Partial Eta Squared
  • Downloading and Installing G*Power: Windows/PC
  • Correlation
  • Testing Parametric Assumptions
  • One-Way ANOVA
  • Two-Way ANOVA
  • Repeated Measures ANOVA
  • Goodness-of-Fit
  • Test of Association
  • Pearson's r
  • Point Biserial
  • Mediation and Moderation
  • Simple Linear Regression
  • Multiple Linear Regression
  • Binomial Logistic Regression
  • Multinomial Logistic Regression
  • Independent Samples T-test
  • Dependent Samples T-test
  • Testing Assumptions
  • T-tests using SPSS
  • T-Test Practice
  • Predictive Analytics This link opens in a new window
  • Quantitative Research Questions
  • Null & Alternative Hypotheses
  • One-Tail vs. Two-Tail
  • Alpha & Beta
  • Associated Probability
  • Decision Rule
  • Statement of Conclusion
  • Statistics Group Sessions

ASC Chat Hours

ASC Chat is usually available at the following times ( Pacific Time):

If there is not a coach on duty, submit your question via one of the below methods:

  928-440-1325

  Ask a Coach

  [email protected]

Search our FAQs on the Academic Success Center's  Ask a Coach   page.

Once you have developed a clear and focused research question or set of research questions, you’ll be ready to conduct further research, a literature review, on the topic to help you make an educated guess about the answer to your question(s). This educated guess is called a hypothesis.

In research, there are two types of hypotheses: null and alternative. They work as a complementary pair, each stating that the other is wrong.

  • Null Hypothesis (H 0 ) – This can be thought of as the implied hypothesis. “Null” meaning “nothing.”  This hypothesis states that there is no difference between groups or no relationship between variables. The null hypothesis is a presumption of status quo or no change.
  • Alternative Hypothesis (H a ) – This is also known as the claim. This hypothesis should state what you expect the data to show, based on your research on the topic. This is your answer to your research question.

Null Hypothesis:   H 0 : There is no difference in the salary of factory workers based on gender. Alternative Hypothesis :  H a : Male factory workers have a higher salary than female factory workers.

Null Hypothesis :  H 0 : There is no relationship between height and shoe size. Alternative Hypothesis :  H a : There is a positive relationship between height and shoe size.

Null Hypothesis :  H 0 : Experience on the job has no impact on the quality of a brick mason’s work. Alternative Hypothesis :  H a : The quality of a brick mason’s work is influenced by on-the-job experience.

Was this resource helpful?

  • << Previous: Hypothesis Testing
  • Next: One-Tail vs. Two-Tail >>
  • Last Updated: Apr 19, 2024 3:09 PM
  • URL: https://resources.nu.edu/statsresources

NCU Library Home

Module 9: Hypothesis Testing With One Sample

Null and alternative hypotheses, learning outcomes.

  • Describe hypothesis testing in general and in practice

The actual test begins by considering two  hypotheses . They are called the null hypothesis and the alternative hypothesis . These hypotheses contain opposing viewpoints.

H 0 : The null hypothesis: It is a statement about the population that either is believed to be true or is used to put forth an argument unless it can be shown to be incorrect beyond a reasonable doubt.

H a : The alternative hypothesis : It is a claim about the population that is contradictory to H 0 and what we conclude when we reject H 0 .

Since the null and alternative hypotheses are contradictory, you must examine evidence to decide if you have enough evidence to reject the null hypothesis or not. The evidence is in the form of sample data.

After you have determined which hypothesis the sample supports, you make adecision. There are two options for a  decision . They are “reject H 0 ” if the sample information favors the alternative hypothesis or “do not reject H 0 ” or “decline to reject H 0 ” if the sample information is insufficient to reject the null hypothesis.

Mathematical Symbols Used in  H 0 and H a :

H 0 always has a symbol with an equal in it. H a never has a symbol with an equal in it. The choice of symbol depends on the wording of the hypothesis test. However, be aware that many researchers (including one of the co-authors in research work) use = in the null hypothesis, even with > or < as the symbol in the alternative hypothesis. This practice is acceptable because we only make the decision to reject or not reject the null hypothesis.

H 0 : No more than 30% of the registered voters in Santa Clara County voted in the primary election. p ≤ 30

H a : More than 30% of the registered voters in Santa Clara County voted in the primary election. p > 30

A medical trial is conducted to test whether or not a new medicine reduces cholesterol by 25%. State the null and alternative hypotheses.

H 0 : The drug reduces cholesterol by 25%. p = 0.25

H a : The drug does not reduce cholesterol by 25%. p ≠ 0.25

We want to test whether the mean GPA of students in American colleges is different from 2.0 (out of 4.0). The null and alternative hypotheses are:

H 0 : μ = 2.0

H a : μ ≠ 2.0

We want to test whether the mean height of eighth graders is 66 inches. State the null and alternative hypotheses. Fill in the correct symbol (=, ≠, ≥, <, ≤, >) for the null and alternative hypotheses. H 0 : μ __ 66 H a : μ __ 66

  • H 0 : μ = 66
  • H a : μ ≠ 66

We want to test if college students take less than five years to graduate from college, on the average. The null and alternative hypotheses are:

H 0 : μ ≥ 5

H a : μ < 5

We want to test if it takes fewer than 45 minutes to teach a lesson plan. State the null and alternative hypotheses. Fill in the correct symbol ( =, ≠, ≥, <, ≤, >) for the null and alternative hypotheses. H 0 : μ __ 45 H a : μ __ 45

  • H 0 : μ ≥ 45
  • H a : μ < 45

In an issue of U.S. News and World Report , an article on school standards stated that about half of all students in France, Germany, and Israel take advanced placement exams and a third pass. The same article stated that 6.6% of U.S. students take advanced placement exams and 4.4% pass. Test if the percentage of U.S. students who take advanced placement exams is more than 6.6%. State the null and alternative hypotheses.

H 0 : p ≤ 0.066

H a : p > 0.066

On a state driver’s test, about 40% pass the test on the first try. We want to test if more than 40% pass on the first try. Fill in the correct symbol (=, ≠, ≥, <, ≤, >) for the null and alternative hypotheses. H 0 : p __ 0.40 H a : p __ 0.40

  • H 0 : p = 0.40
  • H a : p > 0.40

Concept Review

In a  hypothesis test , sample data is evaluated in order to arrive at a decision about some type of claim. If certain conditions about the sample are satisfied, then the claim can be evaluated for a population. In a hypothesis test, we: Evaluate the null hypothesis , typically denoted with H 0 . The null is not rejected unless the hypothesis test shows otherwise. The null statement must always contain some form of equality (=, ≤ or ≥) Always write the alternative hypothesis , typically denoted with H a or H 1 , using less than, greater than, or not equals symbols, i.e., (≠, >, or <). If we reject the null hypothesis, then we can assume there is enough evidence to support the alternative hypothesis. Never state that a claim is proven true or false. Keep in mind the underlying fact that hypothesis testing is based on probability laws; therefore, we can talk only in terms of non-absolute certainties.

Formula Review

H 0 and H a are contradictory.

  • OpenStax, Statistics, Null and Alternative Hypotheses. Provided by : OpenStax. Located at : http://cnx.org/contents/[email protected]:58/Introductory_Statistics . License : CC BY: Attribution
  • Introductory Statistics . Authored by : Barbara Illowski, Susan Dean. Provided by : Open Stax. Located at : http://cnx.org/contents/[email protected] . License : CC BY: Attribution . License Terms : Download for free at http://cnx.org/contents/[email protected]
  • Simple hypothesis testing | Probability and Statistics | Khan Academy. Authored by : Khan Academy. Located at : https://youtu.be/5D1gV37bKXY . License : All Rights Reserved . License Terms : Standard YouTube License

Our websites may use cookies to personalize and enhance your experience. By continuing without changing your cookie settings, you agree to this collection. For more information, please see our University Websites Privacy Notice .

Neag School of Education

Educational Research Basics by Del Siegle

Null and alternative hypotheses.

Converting research questions to hypothesis is a simple task. Take the questions and make it a positive statement that says a relationship exists (correlation studies) or a difference exists between the groups (experiment study) and you have the alternative hypothesis. Write the statement such that a relationship does not exist or a difference does not exist and you have the null hypothesis. You can reverse the process if you have a hypothesis and wish to write a research question.

When you are comparing two groups, the groups are the independent variable. When you are testing whether something affects something else, the cause is the independent variable. The independent variable is the one you manipulate.

Teachers given higher pay will have more positive attitudes toward children than teachers given lower pay. The first step is to ask yourself “Are there two or more groups being compared?” The answer is “Yes.” What are the groups? Teachers who are given higher pay and teachers who are given lower pay. The independent variable is teacher pay. The dependent variable (the outcome) is attitude towards school.

You could also approach is another way. “Is something causing something else?” The answer is “Yes.”  What is causing what? Teacher pay is causing attitude towards school. Therefore, teacher pay is the independent variable (cause) and attitude towards school is the dependent variable (outcome).

By tradition, we try to disprove (reject) the null hypothesis. We can never prove a null hypothesis, because it is impossible to prove something does not exist. We can disprove something does not exist by finding an example of it. Therefore, in research we try to disprove the null hypothesis. When we do find that a relationship (or difference) exists then we reject the null and accept the alternative. If we do not find that a relationship (or difference) exists, we fail to reject the null hypothesis (and go with it). We never say we accept the null hypothesis because it is never possible to prove something does not exist. That is why we say that we failed to reject the null hypothesis, rather than we accepted it.

Del Siegle, Ph.D. Neag School of Education – University of Connecticut [email protected] www.delsiegle.com

Library homepage

  • school Campus Bookshelves
  • menu_book Bookshelves
  • perm_media Learning Objects
  • login Login
  • how_to_reg Request Instructor Account
  • hub Instructor Commons
  • Download Page (PDF)
  • Download Full Book (PDF)
  • Periodic Table
  • Physics Constants
  • Scientific Calculator
  • Reference & Cite
  • Tools expand_more
  • Readability

selected template will load here

This action is not available.

Statistics LibreTexts

9.2: Null and Alternative Hypotheses

  • Last updated
  • Save as PDF
  • Page ID 771

The actual test begins by considering two hypotheses . They are called the null hypothesis and the alternative hypothesis . These hypotheses contain opposing viewpoints.

\(H_0\): The null hypothesis: It is a statement of no difference between the variables—they are not related. This can often be considered the status quo and as a result if you cannot accept the null it requires some action.

\(H_a\): The alternative hypothesis: It is a claim about the population that is contradictory to \(H_0\) and what we conclude when we reject \(H_0\). This is usually what the researcher is trying to prove.

Since the null and alternative hypotheses are contradictory, you must examine evidence to decide if you have enough evidence to reject the null hypothesis or not. The evidence is in the form of sample data.

After you have determined which hypothesis the sample supports, you make a decision. There are two options for a decision. They are "reject \(H_0\)" if the sample information favors the alternative hypothesis or "do not reject \(H_0\)" or "decline to reject \(H_0\)" if the sample information is insufficient to reject the null hypothesis.

\(H_{0}\) always has a symbol with an equal in it. \(H_{a}\) never has a symbol with an equal in it. The choice of symbol depends on the wording of the hypothesis test. However, be aware that many researchers (including one of the co-authors in research work) use = in the null hypothesis, even with > or < as the symbol in the alternative hypothesis. This practice is acceptable because we only make the decision to reject or not reject the null hypothesis.

Example \(\PageIndex{1}\)

  • \(H_{0}\): No more than 30% of the registered voters in Santa Clara County voted in the primary election. \(p \leq 30\)
  • \(H_{a}\): More than 30% of the registered voters in Santa Clara County voted in the primary election. \(p > 30\)

Exercise \(\PageIndex{1}\)

A medical trial is conducted to test whether or not a new medicine reduces cholesterol by 25%. State the null and alternative hypotheses.

  • \(H_{0}\): The drug reduces cholesterol by 25%. \(p = 0.25\)
  • \(H_{a}\): The drug does not reduce cholesterol by 25%. \(p \neq 0.25\)

Example \(\PageIndex{2}\)

We want to test whether the mean GPA of students in American colleges is different from 2.0 (out of 4.0). The null and alternative hypotheses are:

  • \(H_{0}: \mu = 2.0\)
  • \(H_{a}: \mu \neq 2.0\)

Exercise \(\PageIndex{2}\)

We want to test whether the mean height of eighth graders is 66 inches. State the null and alternative hypotheses. Fill in the correct symbol \((=, \neq, \geq, <, \leq, >)\) for the null and alternative hypotheses.

  • \(H_{0}: \mu \_ 66\)
  • \(H_{a}: \mu \_ 66\)
  • \(H_{0}: \mu = 66\)
  • \(H_{a}: \mu \neq 66\)

Example \(\PageIndex{3}\)

We want to test if college students take less than five years to graduate from college, on the average. The null and alternative hypotheses are:

  • \(H_{0}: \mu \geq 5\)
  • \(H_{a}: \mu < 5\)

Exercise \(\PageIndex{3}\)

We want to test if it takes fewer than 45 minutes to teach a lesson plan. State the null and alternative hypotheses. Fill in the correct symbol ( =, ≠, ≥, <, ≤, >) for the null and alternative hypotheses.

  • \(H_{0}: \mu \_ 45\)
  • \(H_{a}: \mu \_ 45\)
  • \(H_{0}: \mu \geq 45\)
  • \(H_{a}: \mu < 45\)

Example \(\PageIndex{4}\)

In an issue of U. S. News and World Report , an article on school standards stated that about half of all students in France, Germany, and Israel take advanced placement exams and a third pass. The same article stated that 6.6% of U.S. students take advanced placement exams and 4.4% pass. Test if the percentage of U.S. students who take advanced placement exams is more than 6.6%. State the null and alternative hypotheses.

  • \(H_{0}: p \leq 0.066\)
  • \(H_{a}: p > 0.066\)

Exercise \(\PageIndex{4}\)

On a state driver’s test, about 40% pass the test on the first try. We want to test if more than 40% pass on the first try. Fill in the correct symbol (\(=, \neq, \geq, <, \leq, >\)) for the null and alternative hypotheses.

  • \(H_{0}: p \_ 0.40\)
  • \(H_{a}: p \_ 0.40\)
  • \(H_{0}: p = 0.40\)
  • \(H_{a}: p > 0.40\)

COLLABORATIVE EXERCISE

Bring to class a newspaper, some news magazines, and some Internet articles . In groups, find articles from which your group can write null and alternative hypotheses. Discuss your hypotheses with the rest of the class.

In a hypothesis test , sample data is evaluated in order to arrive at a decision about some type of claim. If certain conditions about the sample are satisfied, then the claim can be evaluated for a population. In a hypothesis test, we:

  • Evaluate the null hypothesis , typically denoted with \(H_{0}\). The null is not rejected unless the hypothesis test shows otherwise. The null statement must always contain some form of equality \((=, \leq \text{or} \geq)\)
  • Always write the alternative hypothesis , typically denoted with \(H_{a}\) or \(H_{1}\), using less than, greater than, or not equals symbols, i.e., \((\neq, >, \text{or} <)\).
  • If we reject the null hypothesis, then we can assume there is enough evidence to support the alternative hypothesis.
  • Never state that a claim is proven true or false. Keep in mind the underlying fact that hypothesis testing is based on probability laws; therefore, we can talk only in terms of non-absolute certainties.

Formula Review

\(H_{0}\) and \(H_{a}\) are contradictory.

  • If \(\alpha \leq p\)-value, then do not reject \(H_{0}\).
  • If\(\alpha > p\)-value, then reject \(H_{0}\).

\(\alpha\) is preconceived. Its value is set before the hypothesis test starts. The \(p\)-value is calculated from the data.References

Data from the National Institute of Mental Health. Available online at http://www.nimh.nih.gov/publicat/depression.cfm .

Quantitative Research

  • Reference work entry
  • First Online: 13 January 2019
  • Cite this reference work entry

alternative hypothesis in quantitative research

  • Leigh A. Wilson 2 , 3  

4249 Accesses

4 Citations

Quantitative research methods are concerned with the planning, design, and implementation of strategies to collect and analyze data. Descartes, the seventeenth-century philosopher, suggested that how the results are achieved is often more important than the results themselves, as the journey taken along the research path is a journey of discovery. High-quality quantitative research is characterized by the attention given to the methods and the reliability of the tools used to collect the data. The ability to critique research in a systematic way is an essential component of a health professional’s role in order to deliver high quality, evidence-based healthcare. This chapter is intended to provide a simple overview of the way new researchers and health practitioners can understand and employ quantitative methods. The chapter offers practical, realistic guidance in a learner-friendly way and uses a logical sequence to understand the process of hypothesis development, study design, data collection and handling, and finally data analysis and interpretation.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Babbie ER. The practice of social research. 14th ed. Belmont: Wadsworth Cengage; 2016.

Google Scholar  

Descartes. Cited in Halverston, W. (1976). In: A concise introduction to philosophy, 3rd ed. New York: Random House; 1637.

Doll R, Hill AB. The mortality of doctors in relation to their smoking habits. BMJ. 1954;328(7455):1529–33. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.328.7455.1529 .

Article   Google Scholar  

Liamputtong P. Research methods in health: foundations for evidence-based practice. 3rd ed. Melbourne: Oxford University Press; 2017.

McNabb DE. Research methods in public administration and nonprofit management: quantitative and qualitative approaches. 2nd ed. New York: Armonk; 2007.

Merriam-Webster. Dictionary. http://www.merriam-webster.com . Accessed 20th December 2017.

Olesen Larsen P, von Ins M. The rate of growth in scientific publication and the decline in coverage provided by Science Citation Index. Scientometrics. 2010;84(3):575–603.

Pannucci CJ, Wilkins EG. Identifying and avoiding bias in research. Plast Reconstr Surg. 2010;126(2):619–25. https://doi.org/10.1097/PRS.0b013e3181de24bc .

Petrie A, Sabin C. Medical statistics at a glance. 2nd ed. London: Blackwell Publishing; 2005.

Portney LG, Watkins MP. Foundations of clinical research: applications to practice. 3rd ed. New Jersey: Pearson Publishing; 2009.

Sheehan J. Aspects of research methodology. Nurse Educ Today. 1986;6:193–203.

Wilson LA, Black DA. Health, science research and research methods. Sydney: McGraw Hill; 2013.

Download references

Author information

Authors and affiliations.

School of Science and Health, Western Sydney University, Penrith, NSW, Australia

Leigh A. Wilson

Faculty of Health Science, Discipline of Behavioural and Social Sciences in Health, University of Sydney, Lidcombe, NSW, Australia

You can also search for this author in PubMed   Google Scholar

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Leigh A. Wilson .

Editor information

Editors and affiliations.

Pranee Liamputtong

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2019 Springer Nature Singapore Pte Ltd.

About this entry

Cite this entry.

Wilson, L.A. (2019). Quantitative Research. In: Liamputtong, P. (eds) Handbook of Research Methods in Health Social Sciences. Springer, Singapore. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-981-10-5251-4_54

Download citation

DOI : https://doi.org/10.1007/978-981-10-5251-4_54

Published : 13 January 2019

Publisher Name : Springer, Singapore

Print ISBN : 978-981-10-5250-7

Online ISBN : 978-981-10-5251-4

eBook Packages : Social Sciences Reference Module Humanities and Social Sciences Reference Module Business, Economics and Social Sciences

Share this entry

Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content:

Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article.

Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative

  • Publish with us

Policies and ethics

  • Find a journal
  • Track your research

Ohio State nav bar

The Ohio State University

  • BuckeyeLink
  • Find People
  • Search Ohio State

Research Questions & Hypotheses

Generally, in quantitative studies, reviewers expect hypotheses rather than research questions. However, both research questions and hypotheses serve different purposes and can be beneficial when used together.

Research Questions

Clarify the research’s aim (farrugia et al., 2010).

  • Research often begins with an interest in a topic, but a deep understanding of the subject is crucial to formulate an appropriate research question.
  • Descriptive: “What factors most influence the academic achievement of senior high school students?”
  • Comparative: “What is the performance difference between teaching methods A and B?”
  • Relationship-based: “What is the relationship between self-efficacy and academic achievement?”
  • Increasing knowledge about a subject can be achieved through systematic literature reviews, in-depth interviews with patients (and proxies), focus groups, and consultations with field experts.
  • Some funding bodies, like the Canadian Institute for Health Research, recommend conducting a systematic review or a pilot study before seeking grants for full trials.
  • The presence of multiple research questions in a study can complicate the design, statistical analysis, and feasibility.
  • It’s advisable to focus on a single primary research question for the study.
  • The primary question, clearly stated at the end of a grant proposal’s introduction, usually specifies the study population, intervention, and other relevant factors.
  • The FINER criteria underscore aspects that can enhance the chances of a successful research project, including specifying the population of interest, aligning with scientific and public interest, clinical relevance, and contribution to the field, while complying with ethical and national research standards.
  • The P ICOT approach is crucial in developing the study’s framework and protocol, influencing inclusion and exclusion criteria and identifying patient groups for inclusion.
  • Defining the specific population, intervention, comparator, and outcome helps in selecting the right outcome measurement tool.
  • The more precise the population definition and stricter the inclusion and exclusion criteria, the more significant the impact on the interpretation, applicability, and generalizability of the research findings.
  • A restricted study population enhances internal validity but may limit the study’s external validity and generalizability to clinical practice.
  • A broadly defined study population may better reflect clinical practice but could increase bias and reduce internal validity.
  • An inadequately formulated research question can negatively impact study design, potentially leading to ineffective outcomes and affecting publication prospects.

Checklist: Good research questions for social science projects (Panke, 2018)

alternative hypothesis in quantitative research

Research Hypotheses

Present the researcher’s predictions based on specific statements.

  • These statements define the research problem or issue and indicate the direction of the researcher’s predictions.
  • Formulating the research question and hypothesis from existing data (e.g., a database) can lead to multiple statistical comparisons and potentially spurious findings due to chance.
  • The research or clinical hypothesis, derived from the research question, shapes the study’s key elements: sampling strategy, intervention, comparison, and outcome variables.
  • Hypotheses can express a single outcome or multiple outcomes.
  • After statistical testing, the null hypothesis is either rejected or not rejected based on whether the study’s findings are statistically significant.
  • Hypothesis testing helps determine if observed findings are due to true differences and not chance.
  • Hypotheses can be 1-sided (specific direction of difference) or 2-sided (presence of a difference without specifying direction).
  • 2-sided hypotheses are generally preferred unless there’s a strong justification for a 1-sided hypothesis.
  • A solid research hypothesis, informed by a good research question, influences the research design and paves the way for defining clear research objectives.

Types of Research Hypothesis

  • In a Y-centered research design, the focus is on the dependent variable (DV) which is specified in the research question. Theories are then used to identify independent variables (IV) and explain their causal relationship with the DV.
  • Example: “An increase in teacher-led instructional time (IV) is likely to improve student reading comprehension scores (DV), because extensive guided practice under expert supervision enhances learning retention and skill mastery.”
  • Hypothesis Explanation: The dependent variable (student reading comprehension scores) is the focus, and the hypothesis explores how changes in the independent variable (teacher-led instructional time) affect it.
  • In X-centered research designs, the independent variable is specified in the research question. Theories are used to determine potential dependent variables and the causal mechanisms at play.
  • Example: “Implementing technology-based learning tools (IV) is likely to enhance student engagement in the classroom (DV), because interactive and multimedia content increases student interest and participation.”
  • Hypothesis Explanation: The independent variable (technology-based learning tools) is the focus, with the hypothesis exploring its impact on a potential dependent variable (student engagement).
  • Probabilistic hypotheses suggest that changes in the independent variable are likely to lead to changes in the dependent variable in a predictable manner, but not with absolute certainty.
  • Example: “The more teachers engage in professional development programs (IV), the more their teaching effectiveness (DV) is likely to improve, because continuous training updates pedagogical skills and knowledge.”
  • Hypothesis Explanation: This hypothesis implies a probable relationship between the extent of professional development (IV) and teaching effectiveness (DV).
  • Deterministic hypotheses state that a specific change in the independent variable will lead to a specific change in the dependent variable, implying a more direct and certain relationship.
  • Example: “If the school curriculum changes from traditional lecture-based methods to project-based learning (IV), then student collaboration skills (DV) are expected to improve because project-based learning inherently requires teamwork and peer interaction.”
  • Hypothesis Explanation: This hypothesis presumes a direct and definite outcome (improvement in collaboration skills) resulting from a specific change in the teaching method.
  • Example : “Students who identify as visual learners will score higher on tests that are presented in a visually rich format compared to tests presented in a text-only format.”
  • Explanation : This hypothesis aims to describe the potential difference in test scores between visual learners taking visually rich tests and text-only tests, without implying a direct cause-and-effect relationship.
  • Example : “Teaching method A will improve student performance more than method B.”
  • Explanation : This hypothesis compares the effectiveness of two different teaching methods, suggesting that one will lead to better student performance than the other. It implies a direct comparison but does not necessarily establish a causal mechanism.
  • Example : “Students with higher self-efficacy will show higher levels of academic achievement.”
  • Explanation : This hypothesis predicts a relationship between the variable of self-efficacy and academic achievement. Unlike a causal hypothesis, it does not necessarily suggest that one variable causes changes in the other, but rather that they are related in some way.

Tips for developing research questions and hypotheses for research studies

  • Perform a systematic literature review (if one has not been done) to increase knowledge and familiarity with the topic and to assist with research development.
  • Learn about current trends and technological advances on the topic.
  • Seek careful input from experts, mentors, colleagues, and collaborators to refine your research question as this will aid in developing the research question and guide the research study.
  • Use the FINER criteria in the development of the research question.
  • Ensure that the research question follows PICOT format.
  • Develop a research hypothesis from the research question.
  • Ensure that the research question and objectives are answerable, feasible, and clinically relevant.

If your research hypotheses are derived from your research questions, particularly when multiple hypotheses address a single question, it’s recommended to use both research questions and hypotheses. However, if this isn’t the case, using hypotheses over research questions is advised. It’s important to note these are general guidelines, not strict rules. If you opt not to use hypotheses, consult with your supervisor for the best approach.

Farrugia, P., Petrisor, B. A., Farrokhyar, F., & Bhandari, M. (2010). Practical tips for surgical research: Research questions, hypotheses and objectives.  Canadian journal of surgery. Journal canadien de chirurgie ,  53 (4), 278–281.

Hulley, S. B., Cummings, S. R., Browner, W. S., Grady, D., & Newman, T. B. (2007). Designing clinical research. Philadelphia.

Panke, D. (2018). Research design & method selection: Making good choices in the social sciences.  Research Design & Method Selection , 1-368.

User Preferences

Content preview.

Arcu felis bibendum ut tristique et egestas quis:

  • Ut enim ad minim veniam, quis nostrud exercitation ullamco laboris
  • Duis aute irure dolor in reprehenderit in voluptate
  • Excepteur sint occaecat cupidatat non proident

Keyboard Shortcuts

10.1 - setting the hypotheses: examples.

A significance test examines whether the null hypothesis provides a plausible explanation of the data. The null hypothesis itself does not involve the data. It is a statement about a parameter (a numerical characteristic of the population). These population values might be proportions or means or differences between means or proportions or correlations or odds ratios or any other numerical summary of the population. The alternative hypothesis is typically the research hypothesis of interest. Here are some examples.

Example 10.2: Hypotheses with One Sample of One Categorical Variable Section  

About 10% of the human population is left-handed. Suppose a researcher at Penn State speculates that students in the College of Arts and Architecture are more likely to be left-handed than people found in the general population. We only have one sample since we will be comparing a population proportion based on a sample value to a known population value.

  • Research Question : Are artists more likely to be left-handed than people found in the general population?
  • Response Variable : Classification of the student as either right-handed or left-handed

State Null and Alternative Hypotheses

  • Null Hypothesis : Students in the College of Arts and Architecture are no more likely to be left-handed than people in the general population (population percent of left-handed students in the College of Art and Architecture = 10% or p = .10).
  • Alternative Hypothesis : Students in the College of Arts and Architecture are more likely to be left-handed than people in the general population (population percent of left-handed students in the College of Arts and Architecture > 10% or p > .10). This is a one-sided alternative hypothesis.

Example 10.3: Hypotheses with One Sample of One Measurement Variable Section  

 two Diphenhydramine pills

A generic brand of the anti-histamine Diphenhydramine markets a capsule with a 50 milligram dose. The manufacturer is worried that the machine that fills the capsules has come out of calibration and is no longer creating capsules with the appropriate dosage.

  • Research Question : Does the data suggest that the population mean dosage of this brand is different than 50 mg?
  • Response Variable : dosage of the active ingredient found by a chemical assay.
  • Null Hypothesis : On the average, the dosage sold under this brand is 50 mg (population mean dosage = 50 mg).
  • Alternative Hypothesis : On the average, the dosage sold under this brand is not 50 mg (population mean dosage ≠ 50 mg). This is a two-sided alternative hypothesis.

Example 10.4: Hypotheses with Two Samples of One Categorical Variable Section  

vegetarian airline meal

Many people are starting to prefer vegetarian meals on a regular basis. Specifically, a researcher believes that females are more likely than males to eat vegetarian meals on a regular basis.

  • Research Question : Does the data suggest that females are more likely than males to eat vegetarian meals on a regular basis?
  • Response Variable : Classification of whether or not a person eats vegetarian meals on a regular basis
  • Explanatory (Grouping) Variable: Sex
  • Null Hypothesis : There is no sex effect regarding those who eat vegetarian meals on a regular basis (population percent of females who eat vegetarian meals on a regular basis = population percent of males who eat vegetarian meals on a regular basis or p females = p males ).
  • Alternative Hypothesis : Females are more likely than males to eat vegetarian meals on a regular basis (population percent of females who eat vegetarian meals on a regular basis > population percent of males who eat vegetarian meals on a regular basis or p females > p males ). This is a one-sided alternative hypothesis.

Example 10.5: Hypotheses with Two Samples of One Measurement Variable Section  

low carb meal

Obesity is a major health problem today. Research is starting to show that people may be able to lose more weight on a low carbohydrate diet than on a low fat diet.

  • Research Question : Does the data suggest that, on the average, people are able to lose more weight on a low carbohydrate diet than on a low fat diet?
  • Response Variable : Weight loss (pounds)
  • Explanatory (Grouping) Variable : Type of diet
  • Null Hypothesis : There is no difference in the mean amount of weight loss when comparing a low carbohydrate diet with a low fat diet (population mean weight loss on a low carbohydrate diet = population mean weight loss on a low fat diet).
  • Alternative Hypothesis : The mean weight loss should be greater for those on a low carbohydrate diet when compared with those on a low fat diet (population mean weight loss on a low carbohydrate diet > population mean weight loss on a low fat diet). This is a one-sided alternative hypothesis.

Example 10.6: Hypotheses about the relationship between Two Categorical Variables Section  

  • Research Question : Do the odds of having a stroke increase if you inhale second hand smoke ? A case-control study of non-smoking stroke patients and controls of the same age and occupation are asked if someone in their household smokes.
  • Variables : There are two different categorical variables (Stroke patient vs control and whether the subject lives in the same household as a smoker). Living with a smoker (or not) is the natural explanatory variable and having a stroke (or not) is the natural response variable in this situation.
  • Null Hypothesis : There is no relationship between whether or not a person has a stroke and whether or not a person lives with a smoker (odds ratio between stroke and second-hand smoke situation is = 1).
  • Alternative Hypothesis : There is a relationship between whether or not a person has a stroke and whether or not a person lives with a smoker (odds ratio between stroke and second-hand smoke situation is > 1). This is a one-tailed alternative.

This research question might also be addressed like example 11.4 by making the hypotheses about comparing the proportion of stroke patients that live with smokers to the proportion of controls that live with smokers.

Example 10.7: Hypotheses about the relationship between Two Measurement Variables Section  

  • Research Question : A financial analyst believes there might be a positive association between the change in a stock's price and the amount of the stock purchased by non-management employees the previous day (stock trading by management being under "insider-trading" regulatory restrictions).
  • Variables : Daily price change information (the response variable) and previous day stock purchases by non-management employees (explanatory variable). These are two different measurement variables.
  • Null Hypothesis : The correlation between the daily stock price change (\$) and the daily stock purchases by non-management employees (\$) = 0.
  • Alternative Hypothesis : The correlation between the daily stock price change (\$) and the daily stock purchases by non-management employees (\$) > 0. This is a one-sided alternative hypothesis.

Example 10.8: Hypotheses about comparing the relationship between Two Measurement Variables in Two Samples Section  

Calculation of a person's approximate tip for their meal

  • Research Question : Is there a linear relationship between the amount of the bill (\$) at a restaurant and the tip (\$) that was left. Is the strength of this association different for family restaurants than for fine dining restaurants?
  • Variables : There are two different measurement variables. The size of the tip would depend on the size of the bill so the amount of the bill would be the explanatory variable and the size of the tip would be the response variable.
  • Null Hypothesis : The correlation between the amount of the bill (\$) at a restaurant and the tip (\$) that was left is the same at family restaurants as it is at fine dining restaurants.
  • Alternative Hypothesis : The correlation between the amount of the bill (\$) at a restaurant and the tip (\$) that was left is the difference at family restaurants then it is at fine dining restaurants. This is a two-sided alternative hypothesis.

Hypothesis Testing

When you conduct a piece of quantitative research, you are inevitably attempting to answer a research question or hypothesis that you have set. One method of evaluating this research question is via a process called hypothesis testing , which is sometimes also referred to as significance testing . Since there are many facets to hypothesis testing, we start with the example we refer to throughout this guide.

An example of a lecturer's dilemma

Two statistics lecturers, Sarah and Mike, think that they use the best method to teach their students. Each lecturer has 50 statistics students who are studying a graduate degree in management. In Sarah's class, students have to attend one lecture and one seminar class every week, whilst in Mike's class students only have to attend one lecture. Sarah thinks that seminars, in addition to lectures, are an important teaching method in statistics, whilst Mike believes that lectures are sufficient by themselves and thinks that students are better off solving problems by themselves in their own time. This is the first year that Sarah has given seminars, but since they take up a lot of her time, she wants to make sure that she is not wasting her time and that seminars improve her students' performance.

The research hypothesis

The first step in hypothesis testing is to set a research hypothesis. In Sarah and Mike's study, the aim is to examine the effect that two different teaching methods – providing both lectures and seminar classes (Sarah), and providing lectures by themselves (Mike) – had on the performance of Sarah's 50 students and Mike's 50 students. More specifically, they want to determine whether performance is different between the two different teaching methods. Whilst Mike is skeptical about the effectiveness of seminars, Sarah clearly believes that giving seminars in addition to lectures helps her students do better than those in Mike's class. This leads to the following research hypothesis:

Before moving onto the second step of the hypothesis testing process, we need to take you on a brief detour to explain why you need to run hypothesis testing at all. This is explained next.

Sample to population

If you have measured individuals (or any other type of "object") in a study and want to understand differences (or any other type of effect), you can simply summarize the data you have collected. For example, if Sarah and Mike wanted to know which teaching method was the best, they could simply compare the performance achieved by the two groups of students – the group of students that took lectures and seminar classes, and the group of students that took lectures by themselves – and conclude that the best method was the teaching method which resulted in the highest performance. However, this is generally of only limited appeal because the conclusions could only apply to students in this study. However, if those students were representative of all statistics students on a graduate management degree, the study would have wider appeal.

In statistics terminology, the students in the study are the sample and the larger group they represent (i.e., all statistics students on a graduate management degree) is called the population . Given that the sample of statistics students in the study are representative of a larger population of statistics students, you can use hypothesis testing to understand whether any differences or effects discovered in the study exist in the population. In layman's terms, hypothesis testing is used to establish whether a research hypothesis extends beyond those individuals examined in a single study.

Another example could be taking a sample of 200 breast cancer sufferers in order to test a new drug that is designed to eradicate this type of cancer. As much as you are interested in helping these specific 200 cancer sufferers, your real goal is to establish that the drug works in the population (i.e., all breast cancer sufferers).

As such, by taking a hypothesis testing approach, Sarah and Mike want to generalize their results to a population rather than just the students in their sample. However, in order to use hypothesis testing, you need to re-state your research hypothesis as a null and alternative hypothesis. Before you can do this, it is best to consider the process/structure involved in hypothesis testing and what you are measuring. This structure is presented on the next page .

If you're seeing this message, it means we're having trouble loading external resources on our website.

If you're behind a web filter, please make sure that the domains *.kastatic.org and *.kasandbox.org are unblocked.

To log in and use all the features of Khan Academy, please enable JavaScript in your browser.

AP®︎/College Statistics

Course: ap®︎/college statistics   >   unit 10.

  • Idea behind hypothesis testing

Examples of null and alternative hypotheses

  • Writing null and alternative hypotheses
  • P-values and significance tests
  • Comparing P-values to different significance levels
  • Estimating a P-value from a simulation
  • Estimating P-values from simulations
  • Using P-values to make conclusions

Want to join the conversation?

  • Upvote Button navigates to signup page
  • Downvote Button navigates to signup page
  • Flag Button navigates to signup page

Good Answer

Video transcript

How to Write a Null and Alternative Hypothesis: A Guide with Examples

11 December 2023

last updated

When undertaking a qualitative or quantitative research project, researchers must first formulate a research question, from which they develop a hypothesis. By definition, a hypothesis is a prediction that a researcher makes about the research question and can either be affirmative or negative. In this case, a research question has three main components: variables (independent and dependent), a population sample, and the relation between the variables. When the prediction contradicts the research question, it is referred to as a null hypothesis. In short, a null hypothesis is a statement that implies there is no relationship between independent and dependent variables. Hence, researchers need to learn how to write a good null and alternative hypothesis to present quality studies.

General Aspect of Writing a Null and Alternative Hypothesis

Students with qualitative or quantitative research assignments must learn how to formulate and write a good research question and hypothesis. By definition, a hypothesis is an assumption or prediction that a researcher makes before undertaking an experimental investigation. Basically, academic standards require such a prediction to be a precise and testable statement, meaning that researchers must prove or disapprove of it in the course of the assignment. In this case, the main components of a hypothesis are variables (independent and dependent), a population sample, and the relation between the variables. Therefore, a research hypothesis is a prediction that researchers write about the relationship between two or more variables. In turn, the research inquiry is the process that seeks to answer the research question and, in the process, test the hypothesis by confirming or disapproving it.

How to write a null and alternative hypothesis

Types of Hypotheses

There are several types of hypotheses, including an alternative hypothesis, a null hypothesis, a directional hypothesis, and a non-directional hypothesis. Basically, the directional hypothesis is a prediction of how the independent variable affects the dependent variable. In contrast, the non-directional hypothesis predicts that the independent variable influences the dependent variable, but does not specify how. Regardless of the type, all hypotheses are about predicting the relationship between the independent and dependent variables.

What Is a Null and Alternative Hypothesis

A null hypothesis, usually symbolized as “H0,” is a statement that contradicts the research hypothesis. In other words, it is a negative statement, indicating that there is no relationship between the independent and dependent variables. By testing the null hypothesis, a researcher can determine whether the inquiry results are due to the chance or the effect of manipulating the dependent variable. In most instances, a null hypothesis corresponds with an alternative hypothesis, a positive statement that covers a relationship that exists between the independent and dependent variables. Also, it is highly recommendable that a researcher should write the alternative hypothesis first before the null hypothesis.

10 Examples of Research Questions with H0 and H1 Hypotheses

Before developing a hypothesis, a researcher must formulate the research question. Then, the next step is to transform the question into a negative statement that claims the lack of a relationship between the independent and dependent variables. Alternatively, researchers can change the question into a positive statement that includes a relationship that exists between the variables. In turn, this latter statement becomes the alternative hypothesis and is symbolized as H1. Hence, some of the examples of research questions and hull and alternative hypotheses are as follows:

1. Do physical exercises help individuals to age gracefully?

A Null Hypothesis (H0): Physical exercises are not a guarantee for graceful old age.

An Alternative Hypothesis (H1): Engaging in physical exercises enables individuals to remain healthy and active into old age.

2. What are the implications of therapeutic interventions in the fight against substance abuse?

H0: Therapeutic interventions are of no help in the fight against substance abuse.

H1: Exposing individuals with substance abuse disorders to therapeutic interventions help control and even stop their addictions.

3. How do sexual orientation and gender identity affect the experiences of late adolescents in foster care?

H0: Sexual orientation and gender identity have no effects on the experiences of late adolescents in foster care.

H1: The reality of stereotypes in society makes sexual orientation and gender identity factors complicate the experiences of late adolescents in foster care.

4. Does income inequality contribute to crime in high-density urban areas?

H0: There is no correlation between income inequality and incidences of crime in high-density urban areas.

H1: The high crime rates in high-density urban areas are due to the incidence of income inequality in those areas.

5. Does placement in foster care impact individuals’ mental health?

H0: There is no correlation between being in foster care and having mental health problems.

H1: Individuals placed in foster care experience anxiety and depression at one point in their life.

6. Do assistive devices and technologies lessen the mobility challenges of older adults with a stroke?

H0: Assistive devices and technologies do not provide any assistance to the mobility of older adults diagnosed with a stroke.

H1: Assistive devices and technologies enhance the mobility of older adults diagnosed with a stroke.

7. Does race identity undermine classroom participation?

H0: There is no correlation between racial identity and the ability to participate in classroom learning.

H1: Students from racial minorities are not as active as white students in classroom participation.

8. Do high school grades determine future success?

H0: There is no correlation between how one performs in high school and their success level in life.

H1: Attaining high grades in high school positions one for greater success in the future personal and professional lives.

9. Does critical thinking predict academic achievement?

H0: There is no correlation between critical thinking and academic achievement.

H1: Being a critical thinker is a pathway to academic success.

10. What benefits does group therapy provide to victims of domestic violence?

H0: Group therapy does not help victims of domestic violence because individuals prefer to hide rather than expose their shame.

H1: Group therapy provides domestic violence victims with a platform to share their hurt and connect with others with similar experiences.

Summing Up on How to Write a Null and Alternative Hypothesis

The formulation of research questions in qualitative and quantitative assignments helps students develop a hypothesis for their experiment. In this case, learning how to write a good hypothesis that helps students and researchers to make their research relevant. Basically, the difference between a null and alternative hypothesis is that the former contradicts the research question, while the latter affirms it. In short, a null hypothesis is a negative statement relative to the research question, and an alternative hypothesis is a positive statement. Moreover, it is important to note that developing the null hypothesis at the beginning of the assignment is for prediction purposes. As such, the research work answers the research question and confirms or disapproves of the hypothesis. Hence, some of the tips that students and researchers need to know when developing a null hypothesis include:

  • Formulate a research question that specifies the relationship between an independent variable and a dependent variable.
  • Develop an alternative hypothesis that says a relationship that exists between the variables.
  • Develop a null hypothesis that says a relationship that does not exist between the variables.
  • Conduct the research to answer the research question, which allows the confirmation of a disapproval of a null hypothesis.

To Learn More, Read Relevant Articles

Sat essay examples with explanations and recommendations, how to cite lecture notes in apa: basic guidelines.

U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

The .gov means it’s official. Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

The site is secure. The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

  • Publications
  • Account settings

Preview improvements coming to the PMC website in October 2024. Learn More or Try it out now .

  • Advanced Search
  • Journal List

Logo of plosone

Why we habitually engage in null-hypothesis significance testing: A qualitative study

Jonah stunt.

1 Department of Health Sciences, Section of Methodology and Applied Statistics, Vrije Universiteit, Amsterdam, The Netherlands

2 Department of Radiation Oncology, Erasmus Medical Center, Rotterdam, The Netherlands

Leonie van Grootel

3 Rathenau Institute, The Hague, The Netherlands

4 Department of Philosophy, Vrije Universiteit, Amsterdam, The Netherlands

5 Department of Epidemiology and Data Science, Amsterdam University Medical Centers, Amsterdam, The Netherlands

David Trafimow

6 Psychology Department, New Mexico State University, Las Cruces, New Mexico, United States of America

Trynke Hoekstra

Michiel de boer.

7 Department of General Practice and Elderly Care, University Medical Center Groningen, Groningen, The Netherlands

Associated Data

A full study protocol, including a detailed data analysis plan, was preregistered ( https://osf.io/4qg38/ ). At the start of this study, preregistration forms for qualitative studies were not developed yet. Therefore, preregistration for this study is based on an outdated form. Presently, there is a preregistration form available for qualitative studies. Information about data collection, data management, data sharing and data storage is described in a Data Management Plan. Sensitive data is stored in Darkstor, an offline archive for storing sensitive information or data (information that involves i.e., privacy or copyright). As the recordings and transcripts of the interviews and focus groups contain privacy-sensitive data, these files are archived in Darkstor and can be accessed only on request by authorized individuals (i.e., the original researcher or a research coordinator)1. Non-sensitive data is stored in DANS ( https://doi.org/10.17026/dans-2at-nzfs ) (Data Archiving and Networked Services; the Netherlands institute for permanent access to digital research resources). 1. Data requests can be send to ln.uv@mdr .

Null Hypothesis Significance Testing (NHST) is the most familiar statistical procedure for making inferences about population effects. Important problems associated with this method have been addressed and various alternatives that overcome these problems have been developed. Despite its many well-documented drawbacks, NHST remains the prevailing method for drawing conclusions from data. Reasons for this have been insufficiently investigated. Therefore, the aim of our study was to explore the perceived barriers and facilitators related to the use of NHST and alternative statistical procedures among relevant stakeholders in the scientific system.

Individual semi-structured interviews and focus groups were conducted with junior and senior researchers, lecturers in statistics, editors of scientific journals and program leaders of funding agencies. During the focus groups, important themes that emerged from the interviews were discussed. Data analysis was performed using the constant comparison method, allowing emerging (sub)themes to be fully explored. A theory substantiating the prevailing use of NHST was developed based on the main themes and subthemes we identified.

Twenty-nine interviews and six focus groups were conducted. Several interrelated facilitators and barriers associated with the use of NHST and alternative statistical procedures were identified. These factors were subsumed under three main themes: the scientific climate, scientific duty, and reactivity. As a result of the factors, most participants feel dependent in their actions upon others, have become reactive, and await action and initiatives from others. This may explain why NHST is still the standard and ubiquitously used by almost everyone involved.

Our findings demonstrate how perceived barriers to shift away from NHST set a high threshold for actual behavioral change and create a circle of interdependency between stakeholders. By taking small steps it should be possible to decrease the scientific community’s strong dependence on NHST and p-values.

Introduction

Empirical studies often start from the idea that there might be an association between a specific factor and a certain outcome within a population. This idea is referred to as the alternative hypothesis (H1). Its complement, the null hypothesis (H0), typically assumes no association or effect (although it is possible to test other effect sizes than no effect with the null hypothesis). At the stage of data-analysis, the probability of obtaining the observed, or a more extreme, association is calculated under the assumption of no effect in the population (H0) and a number of inferential assumptions [ 1 ]. The probability of obtaining the observed, or more extreme, data is known as ‘the p-value’. The p-value demonstrates the compatibility between the observed data and the expected data under the null hypothesis, where 0 is complete incompatibility and 1 is perfect compatibility [ 2 ]. When the p-value is smaller than a prespecified value (labelled as alpha, usually set at 5% (0.05)), results are generally declared to be statistically significant. At this point, researchers commonly reject the null hypothesis and accept the alternative hypothesis [ 2 ]. Assessing statistical significance by means of contrasting the data with the null hypothesis is called Null Hypothesis Significance Testing (NHST). NHST is the best known and most widely used statistical procedure for making inferences about population effects. The procedure has become the prevailing paradigm in empirical science [ 3 ], and reaching and being able to report statistically significant results has become the ultimate goal for many researchers.

Despite its widespread use, NHST and the p-value have been criticized since its inception. Numerous publications have addressed problems associated with NHST and p-values. Arguably the most important drawback is the fact that NHST is a form of indirect or inverse inference: researchers usually want to know if the null or alternative hypothesis can be accepted and use NHST to conclude either way. But with NHST, the probability of a finding, or more extreme findings, given the null hypothesis is calculated [ 4 ]. Ergo, NHST doesn’t tell us what we want to know. In fact, p-values were never meant to serve as a basis to draw conclusions, but as a continuous measure of incompatibility between empirical findings and a statistical model [ 2 ]. Moreover, the procedure promotes a dichotomous way of thinking, by using the outcome of a significance test as a dichotomous indicator for an effect (p<0.05: effect, p>0.05: no effect). Reducing empirical findings to two categories also results in a great loss of information. Further, a significant outcome is often unjustly interpreted as relevant, but a p-value does not convey any information about the strength or importance of the association. Worse yet, the p-values on which NHST is based confound effect size and sample size. A trivial effect size may nevertheless result in statistical significance provided a sufficiently large sample size. Or an important effect size may fail to result in statistical significance if the sample size is too small. P-values do not validly index the size, relevance, or precision of an effect [ 5 ]. Furthermore, statistical models include not only null hypotheses, but additional assumptions, some of which are wrong, such as the ubiquitous assumption of random and independent sampling from a defined population [ 1 ]. Therefore, although p-values validly index the incompatibility of data with models, p-values do not validly index incompatibility of data with hypotheses that are embedded in wrong models. These are important drawbacks rendering NHST unsuitable as the default procedure for drawing conclusions from empirical data [ 2 , 3 , 5 – 13 ].

A number of alternatives have been developed that overcome these pitfalls, such as Bayesian inference methods [ 7 , 11 , 14 , 15 ], informative hypothesis testing [ 9 , 16 ] and a priori inferential statistics [ 4 , 17 ]. These alternatives build on the idea that research usually starts with a more informed research-question than one merely assuming the null hypothesis of no effect. These methods overcome the problem of inverse inference, although the first two might still lead to dichotomous thinking with the use of thresholds. Despite the availability of alternatives, statistical behavior in the research community has hardly changed. Researchers have been slow to adopt alternative methods and NHST is still the prevailing paradigm for making inferences about population effects [ 3 ].

Until now, reasons for the continuous and ubiquitous use of NHST and the p-value have scarcely been investigated. One explanation is that NHST provides a very simple means for drawing conclusions from empirical data, usually based on the 5% cut-off. Secondly, most researchers are unaware of the pitfalls of NHST; it has been shown that NHST and the p-value are often misunderstood and misinterpreted [ 2 , 3 , 8 , 11 , 18 , 19 ]. Thirdly, NHST has a central role in most methods and statistics courses in higher education. Courses on alternative methods are increasingly being offered but are usually not mandatory. To our knowledge, there is a lack of in depth, empirical research, aimed at elucidating why NHST nevertheless remains the dominant approach, or what actions can be taken to shift the sciences away from NHST. Therefore, the aim of our study was to explore the perceived barriers and facilitators, as well as behavioral intentions related to the use of NHST and alternatives statistical procedures, among all relevant stakeholders in the scientific system.

Theoretical framework

In designing our study, we used two theories. Firstly, we used the ‘diffusion of innovation theory’ of Rogers [ 20 ]. This theory describes the dissemination of an innovation as a process consisting of four elements: 1) an innovation is 2) communicated through certain channels 3) over time 4) among the members of a social system [ 20 ]. In the current study, the innovation consists of the idea that we should stop with the default use of NHST and instead consider using alternative methods for drawing conclusions from empirical data. The science system forms the social structure in which the innovation should take place. The most important members, and potential adopters of the innovation, we identified are researchers, lecturers, editors of scientific journals and representatives of funding agencies. Rogers describes phases in the adoption process, which coincide with characteristics of the (potential) adopters of the idea: 1) innovators, 2) early adopters, 3) early majority adopters, 4) late majority adopters and 5) laggards. Innovators are the first to adopt an innovation. There are few innovators but these few are very important for bringing in new ideas. Early adopters form the second group to adopt an innovation. This group includes opinion leaders and role models for other stakeholders. The largest group consists of the early and late majority who follow the early adopters, and then there is a smaller group of laggards who resist the innovation until they are certain the innovation will not fail. The process of innovation adoption by individuals is described as a normal distribution ( Fig 1 ). For these five groups, the adoption of a new idea is influenced by the following five characteristics of the innovative idea and 1) its relative advantage, 2) its compatibility with current experiences, 3) its complexity, 4) its flexibility, and 5) its visibility [ 20 ]. Members of all four stakeholder groups could play an important role in the diffusion of the innovation of replacing NHST by its alternatives.

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is pone.0258330.g001.jpg

The innovativeness dimension, measured by the time at which an individual from an adopter category adopts an innovation. Each category is one of more standard deviations removed from the average time of adoption [ 20 ].

Another important theory for our study is the ‘theory of planned behavior’, that was developed in the 1960s [ 21 ]. This theory describes how human behavior in a certain context can be predicted and explained. The theory was updated in 2010, under the name ‘the reasoned action approach’ [ 22 ]. A central factor in this theory is the intention to perform a certain behavior, in this case, to change the default use of NHST. According to the theory, people’s intentions determine their behaviors. An intention indexes to what extent someone is motivated to perform the behavior. Intentions are determined by three independent determinants: the person’s attitudes toward the behavior—the degree to which a person sees the behavior as favorable or unfavorable, perceived subjective norms regarding the behavior—the perceived social pressure to perform the behavior or not, and perceptions of control regarding the behavior—the perceived ease or difficulty of performing the behavior. Underlying (i.e. responsible for) these three constructs are corresponding behavioral, normative, and control beliefs [ 21 , 22 ] (see Fig 2 ).

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is pone.0258330.g002.jpg

Both theories have served as a lens for both data collection and analysis. We used sensitizing concepts [ 23 ] within the framework of the grounded theory approach [ 24 ] from both theories as a starting point for this qualitative study, and more specifically, for the topic list for the interviews and focus groups, providing direction and guidance for the data collection and data analysis.

Many of the concepts of Rogers’ and Fishbein and Ajzen’s theory can be seen as facilitators and barriers for embracing and implementing innovation in the scientific system.

A qualitative study among stakeholders using semi-structured interviews and focus groups was performed. Data collection and analysis were guided by the principle of constant comparison traditional to the grounded theory approach we followed [ 24 ]. The grounded theory is a methodology that uses inductive reasoning, and aims to construct a theory through the collection and analysis of data. Constant comparison is the iterative process whereby each part of the data that emerges from the data analysis is compared with other parts of the data to thoroughly explore and validate the data. Concepts that have been extracted from the data are tagged with codes that are grouped into categories. These categories constitute themes, which (may) become the basis for a new theory. Data collection and analysis were continued until no new information was gained and data saturation had likely occurred within the identified themes.

The target population consisted of stakeholders relevant to our topic: junior and senior researchers, lecturers in statistics, editors of scientific journals and program leaders of funding agencies (see Tables ​ Tables1 1 and ​ and2). 2 ). We approached participants in the field of medical sciences, health- and life sciences and psychology. In line with the grounded theory approach, theoretical sampling was used to identify and recruit eligible participants. Theoretical sampling is a form of purposive sampling. This means that we aimed to purposefully select participants, based on their characteristics that fit the parameters of the research questions [ 25 ]. Recruitment took place by approaching persons in our professional networks and or the networks of the approached persons.

*The numbers between brackets represents the number of participants that were also interviewed.

Data collection

We conducted individual semi-structured interviews followed by focus groups. The aim of the interviews was to gain insight into the views of participants on the use of NHST and alternative methods and to examine potential barriers and facilitators related to these methods. The aim of the focus groups was to validate and further explore interview findings and to develop a comprehensive understanding of participants’ views and beliefs.

For the semi-structured interviews, we used a topic list (see Appendix 1 in S1 Appendix ). Questions addressed participants’ knowledge and beliefs about the concept of NHST, their familiarity with NHST, perceived attractiveness and drawbacks of the use of NHST, knowledge of the current NHST debate, knowledge of and views on alternative procedures and their views on the future of NHST. The topic list was slightly adjusted based on the interviews with editors and representatives from funding agencies (compared to the topic list for interviews with researchers and lecturers). Questions particularly focused on research and education were replaced by questions focused on policy (see Appendix 1 in S1 Appendix ).

The interviews were conducted between October 2017 and June 2018 by two researchers (L.v.G. and J.S.), both trained in qualitative research methods. Interviews lasted about one hour (range 31–86 minutes) and were voice-recorded. One interview was conducted by telephone; all others were face to face and took place at a location convenient for the participants, in most cases the participants’ work location.

Focus groups

During the focus groups, important themes that emerged from the interviews were discussed and explored. These include perceptions on NHST and alternatives and essential conditions to shift away from the default use of NHST.

Five focus groups included representatives from the different stakeholder groups. One focus group was homogenous, including solely lecturers. The focus groups consisted of ‘old’ as well as ‘new’ participants, that is, some of the participants of the focus groups were also in the interview sample. We also selected persons that were open for further contribution to the NHST debate and were willing to help think about (implementing) alternatives for NHST.

The focus groups were conducted between September and December 2018 by three researchers (L.v.G., J.S. and A.d.K.), all trained in qualitative research methods. The focus groups lasted about one-and-a-half hours (range 86–100 minutes).

Data analysis

All interviews and focus groups were transcribed verbatim. Atlas.ti 8.0 software was used for data management and analysis. All transcripts were read thoroughly several times to identify meaningful and relevant text fragments and analyzed by two researchers (J.S. and L.v.G.). Deductive predefined themes and theoretical concepts were used to guide the development of the topic list for the semi-structured interviews and focus groups, and were used as sensitizing concepts [ 23 ] in data collection and data analysis. Inductive themes were identified during the interview process and analysis of the data [ 26 ].

Transcripts were open-, axial- and selectively coded by two researchers (J.S. and L.v.G.). Open coding is the first step in the data-analysis, whereby phenomena found in the text are identified and named (coded). With axial coding, connections between codes are drawn. Selective coding is the process of selecting one central category and relating all other categories to that category, capturing the essence of the research. The constant comparison method [ 27 ] was applied allowing emerging (sub)themes to be fully explored. First, the two researchers independently developed a set of initial codes. Subsequently, findings were discussed until consensus was reached. Codes were then grouped into categories that were covered under subthemes, belonging to main themes. Finally, a theory substantiating the prevailing use of NHST was developed based on the main themes and subthemes.

Ethical issues

This research was conducted in accordance with the Dutch "General Data Protection Regulation" and the “Netherland’s code of conduct for research integrity”. The research protocol had been submitted for review and approved by the ethical review committee of the VU Faculty of Behavioral and Movement Sciences. In addition, the project had been submitted to the Medical Ethics Committee (METC) of the Amsterdam University Medical Centre who decided that the project is not subject to the Medical Research (Human Subjects) Act ( WMO). At the start of data collection, all participants signed an informed consent form.

A full study protocol, including a detailed data analysis plan, was preregistered ( https://osf.io/4qg38/ ). At the start of this study, preregistration forms for qualitative studies were not developed yet. Therefore, preregistration for this study is based on an outdated form. Presently, there is a preregistration form available for qualitative studies [ 28 ]. Information about data collection, data management, data sharing and data storage is described in a Data Management Plan. Sensitive data is stored in Darkstor, an offline archive for storing sensitive information or data (information that involves i.e., privacy or copyright). As the recordings and transcripts of the interviews and focus groups contain privacy-sensitive data, these files are archived in Darkstor and can be accessed only on request by authorized individuals (i.e., the original researcher or a research coordinator) (Data requests can be send to ln.uv@mdr ). Non-sensitive data is stored in DANS ( https://doi.org/10.17026/dans-2at-nzfs ) (Data Archiving and Networked Services; the Netherlands institute for permanent access to digital research resources).

Participant characteristics

Twenty-nine individual interviews and six focus groups were conducted. The focus groups included four to six participants per session. A total of 47 participants were included in the study (13 researchers, 15 lecturers, 11 editors of scientific journals and 8 representatives of funding agencies). Twenty-nine participants were interviewed. Twenty-seven participants took part in the focus group. Nine of the twenty-seven participants were both interviewed and took part in the focus groups. Some participants had multiple roles (i.e., editor and researcher, editor and lecturer or lecturer and researcher) but were classified based on their primary role (assistant professors were classified as lecturers). The lecturers in statistics in our sample were not statisticians themselves. Although they all received training in statistics, they were primarily trained as psychologists, medical doctors, or health scientists. Some lecturers in our sample taught an applied subject, with statistics as part of it. Other lectures taught Methodology and Statistics courses. Statistical skills and knowledge among lecturers varied from modest to quite advanced. Statistical skills and knowledge among participants from the other stakeholder groups varied from poor to quite advanced. All participants were working in the Netherlands. A general overview of the participants is presented in Table 1 . Participant characteristics split up by interviews and focus groups are presented in Table 2 .

Three main themes with sub-themes and categories emerged ( Fig 3 ): the green-colored compartments hold the three main themes: The scientific climate , The scientific duty and Reactivity . Each of these three main themes consists of subthemes, depicted by the yellow-colored compartments. In turn, some (but not all) of the 9 subthemes also have categories. These ‘lower level’ findings are not included in the figure but will be mentioned in the elaboration on the findings and are depicted in Appendix 2 in S1 Appendix . Fig 3 shows how the themes are related to each other. The blue arrows indicate that the themes are interrelated; factors influence each other. The scientific climate affects the way stakeholders perceive and fulfil their scientific duty, the way stakeholders give substance to their scientific duty shapes and maintain the scientific climate. The scientific duty and the scientific climate cause a state of reactivity. Many participants have adopted a ’wait and see’ attitude regarding behavioral changes with respect to statistical methods. They feel dependent on someone else’s action. This leads to a reactive (instead of a proactive) attitude and a low sense of responsibility. ‘Reactivity’ is the core theme, explaining the most critical problem with respect to the continuous and ubiquitous use of NHST.

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is pone.0258330.g003.jpg

Main themes and subthemes are numbered. Categories are mentioned in the body of the text in bold. ‘P’ stands for participant; ‘I’ stands for interviewer.

1. The scientific climate

The theme, ‘the scientific climate’, represents researchers’ (Dutch) perceptions of the many written and unwritten rules they face in the research environment. This theme concerns the opportunities and challenges participants encounter when working in the science system. Dutch academics feel pressured to publish fast and regularly, and to follow conventions and directions of those on whom they depend. They feel this comes at the expense of the quality of their work. Thus, the scientific climate in the Netherlands has a strong influence on the behavior of participants regarding how they set their priorities and control the quality of their work.

1 . 1 Quality control . Monitoring the quality of research is considered very important. Researchers, funding agencies and editors indicate they rely on their own knowledge, expertise, and insight, and those of their colleagues, to guarantee this quality. However, editors or funding agencies are often left with little choice when it comes to compiling an evaluation committee or a review panel. The choice is often like-knows-like-based. Given the limited choice, they are forced to trust the opinion of their consultants, but the question is whether this is justified.

I: “The ones who evaluate the statistics, do they have sufficient statistical knowledge?” P: “Ehhr, no, I don’t think so.” I: “Okay, interesting. So, there are manuscripts published of which you afterwards might think….” P: “Yes yes.” (Interview 18; Professor/editor, Medical Sciences)

1 . 2 Convention . The scientific system is built on mores and conventions, as this participant describes:

P: “There is science, and there is the sociology of science, that is, how we talk to each other, what we believe, how we connect. And at some point, it was agreed upon that we would talk to each other in this way.” (Interview 28, researcher, Medical Sciences)

And to these conventions, one (naturally) conforms. Stakeholders copy behavior and actions of others within their discipline, thereby causing particular behaviors and values to become conventional or normative. One of those conventions is the use of NHST and p-values. Everyone is trained with NHST and is used to applying this method. Another convention is the fact that significant results mean ‘success’, in the sense of successful research and being a successful researcher. Everyone is aware that ‘p is smaller than 0.05’ means the desired results are achieved and that publication and citation chances are increased.

P: “You want to find a significant result so badly. (…) Because people constantly think: I must find a significant result, otherwise my study is worthless.” (Focus group 4, lecturer, Medical Sciences)

Stakeholders rigidly hold on to the above-mentioned conventions and are not inclined to deviate from existing norms; they are, in other words, quite conservative . ‘We don’t know any better’ has been brought up as a valid argument by participants from various stakeholder groups to stick to current rules and conventions. Consequently, the status quo in the scientific system is being maintained.

P: “People hold on to….” I: ‘Everyone maintains the system?’ P: ‘Yes, we kind of hang to the conservative manner. This is what we know, what someone, everyone, accepts.” (Interview 17, researcher, Health Sciences)

Everyone is trained with NHST and considers it an accessible and easy to interpret method. The familiarity and perceived simplicity of NHST, user-friendly software such as SPSS and the clear cut-off value for significance are important facilitators for the use of NHST and at the same time barriers to start using alternative methods. Applied researchers stressed the importance of the accessibility of NHST as a method to test hypotheses and draw conclusions. This accessibility also justifies the use of NHST when researchers want to communicate their study results and messages in understandable ways to their readership.

P: “It is harder, also to explain, to use an alternative. So, I think, but maybe I’m overstepping, but if you want to go in that direction [alternative methods] it needs to be better facilitated for researchers. Because at the moment… I did some research, but, you know, there are those uncommon statistical packages.” (Interview 16, researcher/editor, Medical Sciences)

1 . 3 Publication pressure . Most researchers mentioned that they perceive publication pressure. This motivates them to use NHST and hope for significant results, as ‘significant p-values’ increase publication chances. They perceive a high workload and the way the scientific reward system is constructed as barriers for behavioral change pertaining to the use of statistical methods; potential negative consequences for publication and career chances prevent researchers from deviating from (un)written rules.

P: “I would like to learn it [alternative methods], but it might very well be that I will not be able to apply it, because I will not get my paper published. I find that quite tricky.” (Interview 1, Assistant Professor, Health Sciences)

2. The scientific duty

Throughout the interviews, participants reported a sense of duty in several variations. “What does it mean to be a scientific researcher?” seemed to be a question that was reflected upon during rather than prior to the interview, suggesting that many scientists had not really thought about the moral and professional obligations of being a scientist in general—let alone what that would mean for their use of NHST. Once they had given it some thought, the opinions concerning what constitutes the scientific duty varied to a large extent. Some participants attached great importance to issues such as reproducibility and transparency in scientific research and continuing education and training for researchers. For others, these topics seemed to play a less important role. A distinction was made between moral and professional obligations that participants described concerning their scientific duty.

2 . 1 Moral obligation . The moral obligation concerns issues such as doing research in a thorough and honest way, refraining from questionable research practices (QRPs) and investing in better research. It concerns tasks and activities that are not often rewarded or acknowledged.

Throughout the interviews and the focus groups, participants very frequently touched upon the responsibility they felt for doing ‘the right thing’ and making the right choice in doing research and using NHST, in particular. The extent to which they felt responsible varied among participants. When it comes to choices during doing research—for example, drawing conclusions from data—participants felt a strong sense of responsibility to do this correctly. However, when it comes to innovation and new practices, and feeling responsible for your own research, let alone improving scientific practice in general, opinions differed. This quotation from one of the focus groups illustrates that:

P1: “If you people [statisticians, methodologists] want me to improve the statistics I use in my research, then you have to hand it to me. I am not going to make any effort to improve that myself. “P3: “No. It is your responsibility as an academic to keep growing and learning and so, also to start familiarizing yourself when you notice that your statistics might need improvement.” (Focus group 2, participant 1 (PhD researcher, Medical Sciences) and 3 (Associate Professor, Health Sciences)

The sense of responsibility for improving research practices regarding the use of NHST was strongly felt and emphasized by a small group of participants. They emphasized the responsibility of the researcher to think, interpret and be critical when interpreting the p -value in NHST. It was felt that you cannot leave that up to the reader. Moreover, scrutinizing and reflecting upon research results was considered a primary responsibility of a scientist, and failing to do so, as not living up to what your job demands you to do:

P: “Yes, and if I want to be very provocative—and I often want that, because then people tend to wake up and react: then I say that hiding behind alpha.05 is just scientific laziness. Actually, it is worse: it is scientific cowardice. I would even say it is ‘relieving yourself from your duty’, but that may sound a bit harsh…” (Interview 2, Professor, Health Sciences)

These participants were convinced that scientists have a duty to keep scientific practice in general at the highest level possible.

The avoidance of questionable research practices (QRPs) was considered a means or a way to keep scientific practices high level and was often touched upon during the interviews and focus groups as being part of the scientific duty. Statisticians saw NHST as directly facilitating QRPs and providing ample examples of how the use of NHST leads to QRPs, whereas most applied researchers perceived NHST as the common way of doing research and were not aware of the risks related to QRPs. Participants did mention the violation of assumptions underlying NHST as being a QRP. Then, too, participants considered overinterpreting results as a QRP, including exaggerating the degree of significance. Although participants stated they were careful about interpreting and reporting p-values, they ‘admitted’ that statistical significance was a starting point for them. Most researchers indicated they search for information that could get their study published, which usually includes a low p-value (this also relates to the theme ‘Scientific climate’).

P: “We all know that a lot of weight is given to the p-value. So, if it is not significant, then that’s the end of it. If it ís significant, it just begins.” (Interview 5, lecturer, Psychology)

The term ‘sloppy science’ was mentioned in relation to efforts by researchers to reduce the p -value (a.k.a. p-hacking, data-dredging, and HARKing. HARKing is an acronym that refers to the questionable research question of Hypothesizing After the Results are Known. It occurs when researchers formulate a hypothesis after the data have been collected and analyzed, but make it look like it is an a priori hypothesis [ 29 ]). Preregistration and replication were mentioned as being promising solutions for some of the problems caused by NHST.

2 . 2 . Professional obligation . The theme professional obligation reflects participants’ expressions about what methodological knowledge scientists should have about NHST. In contrast moral obligations, there appeared to be some consensus about scientists’ professional obligations. Participants considered critical evaluation of research results a core professional obligation. Also, within all the stakeholder groups, participants agreed that sufficient statistical knowledge is required for using NHST, but they varied in their insights in the principles, potential and limitations of NHST. This also applied to the extent to which participants were aware of the current debate about NHST.

Participants considered critical thinking as a requirement for fulfilling their professional obligation. It specifically refers to the process of interpreting outcomes and taking all relevant contextual information into consideration. Critical thinking was not only literally referred to by participants, but also emerged by interpreting text fragments on the emphasis within their research. Researchers differed quite strongly in where the emphasis of their research outcomes should be put and what kind of information is required when reporting study results. Participants mentioned the proven effectiveness of a particular treatment, giving a summary of the research results, effect sizes, clinical relevance, p-values, or whether you have made a considerable contribution to science or society.

P: “I come back to the point where I said that people find it arbitrary to state that two points difference on a particular scale is relevant. They prefer to hide behind an alpha of 0.05, as if it is a God given truth, that it counts for one and for all. But it is just as well an invented concept and an invented guideline, an invented cut-off value, that isn’t more objective than other methods?” (Interview 2, Professor, Health Sciences)

For some participants, especially those representing funding agencies, critical thinking was primarily seen as a prerequisite for the utility of the research. The focus, when formulating the research question and interpreting the results, should be on practical relevance and the contribution the research makes to society.

The term ‘ignorance’ arose in the context of the participants’ concern regarding the level of statistical knowledge scientists and other stakeholders have versus what knowledge they should have to adequately apply statistical analysis in their research. The more statistically competent respondents in the sample felt quite strongly about how problematic the lack of knowledge about NHST is among those who regularly use it in their research, let alone the lack of knowledge about alternative methods. They felt that regularly retraining yourself in research methods is an essential part of the professional obligation one has. Applied researchers in the sample agreed that a certain level of background knowledge on NHST was required to apply it properly to research and acknowledged their own ignorance. However, they had different opinions about what level of knowledge is required. Moreover, not all of them regarded it as part of their scientific duty to be informed about all ins and outs of NHST. Some saw it as the responsibility of statisticians to actively inform them (see also the subtheme periphery). Some participants were not aware of their ignorance or stated that some of their colleagues are not aware of their ignorance, i.e., that they are unconsciously incompetent and without realizing it, poorly understood what the p-value and associated outcome measures actually mean.

P: “The worst, and I honestly think that this is the most common, is unconsciously incompetent, people don’t even understand that…” I: “Ignorance.” P: “Yes, but worse, ignorant and not even knowing you are ignorant.” (Interview 2, Professor, Health Sciences)

The lack of proper knowledge about statistical procedures was especially prevalent in the medical sciences. Participants working in or with the medical sciences all confirmed that there is little room for proper statistical training for medical students and that the level of knowledge is fairly low. NHST is often used because of its simplicity. It is especially attractive for medical PhD students because they need their PhD to get ahead in their medical career instead of pursuing a scientific career.

P: “I am not familiar with other ways of doing research. I would really like to learn, but I do not know where I could go. And I do not know whether there are better ways. So sometimes I do read studies of which I think: ‘this is something I could investigate with a completely different test. Apparently, this is also possible, but I don’t know how.’ Yes, there are courses, but I do not know what they are. And here in the medical center, a lot of research is done by medical doctors and these people have hardly been taught any statistics. Maybe they will get one or two statistics courses, they know how to do a t-test and that is about it. (…) And the courses have a very low level of statistics, so to say.” (Interview 1, Assistant Professor, Health Sciences)

Also, the term ‘ awareness ’ arose. Firstly, it refers to being conscious about the limitations of NHST. Secondly, it refers to the awareness of the ongoing discussions about NHST and more broadly, about the replication crisis. The statisticians in the sample emphasized the importance of knowing that NHST has limitations and that it cannot be considered the holy grail of data analysis. They also emphasized the importance of being aware of the debate. A certain level of awareness was considered a necessary requirement for critical thinking. There was variation in that awareness. Some participants were quite informed and were also fairly engaged in the discussion whereas others were very new to the discussion and larger contextual factors, such as the replication crisis.

I: “Are you aware of the debate going on in academia on this topic [NHST]? P: “No, I occasionally see some article sent by a colleague passing by. I have the idea that something is going on, but I do not know how the debate is conducted and how advanced it is. (Interview 6, lecturer, Psychology)

With respect to the theme, ‘the scientific duty’, participants differed to what extent they felt responsible for better and open science, for pioneering, for reviewing, and for growing and learning as a scientist. Participants had one commonality: although they strived for adherence to the norms of good research, the rampant feeling is that this is very difficult, due to the scientific climate. Consequently, participants perceive an internal conflict : a discrepancy between what they want or believe , and what they do . Participants often found themselves struggling with the responsibility they felt they had. Making the scientifically most solid choice was often difficult due to feasibility, time constraints, or certain expectations from supervisors (this is also directly related to the themes ‘Scientific climate’ and ‘Reactivity’). Thus, the scientific climate strongly influences the behavior of scientists regarding how they set their priorities and fulfill their scientific duties. The strong sense of scientific duty was perceived by some participants as a facilitator and by others as a barrier for the use of alternative methods.

3. Reactivity

A consequence of the foregoing factors is that most stakeholders have adopted a reactive attitude and behave accordingly. People are disinclined to take responsibility and await external signals and initiatives of others. This might explain why NHST is being continuously used and remains the default procedure to make inferences about population effects.

The core theme ‘reactivity’ can be explained by the following subthemes and categories:

3 . 1 Periphery . The NHST-problem resides in the periphery in several ways. First, it is a subject that is not given much priority. Secondly, some applied researchers and editors believe that methodological knowledge, as it is not their field of expertise, should not be part of their job requirement. This also applies to the NHST debate. Thirdly, and partly related to the second point, there is a lack of cooperation within and between disciplines.

The term ‘ priority’ was mentioned often when participants were asked to what extent the topic of NHST was subject of discussion in their working environment. Participants indicated that (too) little priority is given to statistics and the problems related to the subject. There is simply a lot going on in their research field and daily work, so there are always more important or urgent issues on the agenda.

P: “Discussions take place in the periphery; many people find it complicated. Or are just a little too busy.” (Interview 5, lecturer, Psychology)

As the NHST debate is not prioritized, initiatives with respect to this issue are not forthcoming. Moreover, researchers and lecturers claim there is neither time nor money available for training in statistics in general or acquiring more insight and skills with respect to (the use of) alternative methods. Busy working schedules were mentioned as an important barrier for improving statistical knowledge and skills.

P: “Well you can use your time once, so it is an issue low on the priority list.” (Focus group 5, researcher, Medical Sciences)

The NHST debate is perceived as the domain of statisticians and methodologists. Also, cooperation between different domains and domain-specific experts is perceived as complicated, as different perceptions and ways of thinking can clash. Therefore, some participants feel that separate worlds should be kept separate; put another way: stick to what you know!

P: “This part is not our job. The editorial staff, we have the assignment to ensure that it is properly written down. But the discussion about that [alternatives], that is outside our territory.” (Interview 26, editor, Medical Sciences)

Within disciplines, individuals tend to act on their own, not being aware that others are working on the same subject and that it would be worthwhile to join forces. The interviews and focus groups exposed that a modest number of participants actively try to change the current situation, but in doing that, feel like lone voices in the wilderness.

P1: “I mean, you become a lone voice in the wilderness.” P2: “Indeed, you don’t want that.” P1: “I get it, but no one listens. There is no audience.” (Focus Group 3, P1: MD, lecturer, medical Sciences, P2: editor, Medical Sciences)

To succeed at positive change, participants emphasized that it is essential that people (interdisciplinary) cooperate and join forces, rather than operate on individual levels, focusing solely on their own working environment.

The caution people show with respect to taking initiative is reenforced by the fear of encountering resistance from their working environment when one voices that change regarding the use of NHST is needed. A condition that was mentioned as essential to bring about change was tactical implementation , that is, taking very small steps. As everyone is still using NHST, taking big steps brings the risk of losing especially the more conservative people along the way. Also, the adjustment of policy, guidelines and educational programs are processes for which we need to provide time and scope.

P: “Everyone still uses it, so I think we have to be more critical, and I think we have to look at some kind of culture change, that means that we are going to let go of it (NHST) more and we will also use other tests, that in the long term will overthrow NHST. I: and what about alternatives? P: I think you should never be too fanatic in those discussion, because then you will provoke resistance. (…) That is not how it works in communication. You will touch them on a sore spot, and they will think: ‘and who are you?’ I: “and what works?” P: “well, gradualness. Tell them to use NHST, do not burn it to the ground, you do not want to touch peoples work, because it is close to their hearts. Instead, you say: ‘try to do another test next to NHST’. Be a pioneer yourself.” (Interview 5, lecturer, Psychology)

3 . 2 . Efficacy . Most participants stated they feel they are not in the position to initiate change. On the one hand, this feeling is related to their hierarchical positions within their working environments. On the other hand, the feeling is caused by the fact that statistics is perceived as a very complex field of expertise and people feel they lack sufficient knowledge and skills, especially about alternative methods.

Many participants stated they felt little sense of empowerment, or self-efficacy. The academic system is perceived as hierarchical, having an unequal balance of power. Most participants believe that it is not in their power to take a lead in innovative actions or to stand up against establishment, and think that this responsibility lies with other stakeholders, that have more status .

P: “Ideally, there would be a kind of an emergency letter from several people whose names open up doors, in which they indicate that in the medical sciences we are throwing away money because research is not being interpreted properly. Well, if these people that we listen to send such an emergency letter to the board of The Netherlands Organization for Health Research and Development [the largest Dutch funding agency for innovation and research in healthcare], I can imagine that this will initiate a discussion.” (…) I: “and with a big name you mean someone from within the science system? P: well, you know, ideally a chairman, or chairmen of the academic medical center. At that level. If they would put a letter together. Yes, that of course would have way more impact. Or some prominent medical doctors, yes, that would have more impact, than if some other person would send a letter yes.” (Interview 19, representative from funding agency, Physical Sciences)

Some participants indicated that they did try to make a difference but encountered too much resistance and therefore gave up their efforts. PhD students feel they have insufficient power to choose their own directions and make their own choices.

P: I am dependent on funding agencies and professors. In the end, I will write a grant application in that direction that gives me the greatest chance of eventually receiving that grant. Not primarily research that I think is the most optimal (…) If I know that reviewers believe the p-value is very important, well, of course I write down a method in which the p-value is central.” (Focus group 2, PhD-student, Medical Sciences)

With a sense of imperturbability, most participants accept that they cannot really change anything.

Lastly, the complexity of the subject is an obstacle for behavioral change. Statistics is perceived as a difficult subject. Participants indicate that they have a lack of knowledge and skills and that they are unsure about their own abilities. This applies to the ‘standard’ statistical methods (NHST), but to a greater extent to alternative methods. Many participants feel that they do not have the capacity to pursue a true understanding of (alternative) statistical methods.

P: “Statistics is just very hard. Time and again, research demonstrates that scientists, even the smartest, have a hard time with statistics.” (Focus group 3, PhD researcher, Psychology)

3 . 3 . Interdependency . As mentioned, participants feel they are not in a sufficiently strong position to take initiative or to behave in an anti-establishment manner. Therefore, they await external signals from people within the scientific system with more status, power, or knowledge. This can be people within their own stakeholder group, or from other stakeholder groups. As a consequence of this attitude, a situation arises in which peoples’ actions largely depend on others. That is, a complex state of interdependency evolves: scientists argue that if the reward system does not change, they are not able to alter their statistical behavior. According to researchers, editors and funding agencies are still very much focused on NHST and especially (significant) p-values, and thus, scientists wait for editors and funders to adjust their policy regarding statistics:

P: “I wrote an article and submitted it to an internal medicine journal. I only mentioned confidence intervals. Then I was asked to also write down the p-values. So, I had to do that. This is how they [editors] can use their power. They decide.” (Interview 1, Assistant Professor, Health Sciences)

Editors and funders in their turn claim they do not maintain a strict policy. Their main position is that scientists should reach consensus about the best statistical procedure, and they will then adjust their policy and guidelines.

P: “We actually believe that the research field itself should direct the quality of its research, and thus, also the discussions.” (Interview 22, representative from funding agency, Neurosciences)

Lecturers, for their part, argue that they cannot revise their educational programs due to the academic system, and university policies are adapted to NHST and p-values.

As most participants seem not to be aware of this process, a circle of interdependency arises that is difficult to break.

P: “Yes, the stupid thing about this perpetual circle is that you are educating people, let’s say in the department of cardiology. They must of course grow, and so they need to publish. If you want to publish you must meet the norms and values of the cardiology journals, so they will write down all those p-values. These people are trained and in twenty years they are on the editorial board of those journals, and then you never get rid of it [the p-value].” (Interview 18, Professor, editor, Medical Sciences)

3 . 4 . Degree of eagerness . Exerting certain behavior or behavioral change is (partly) determined by the extent to which people want to employ particular behavior, their behavioral intention [ 22 ]. Some participants indicated they are willing to change their behavior regarding the use of statistical methods, but only if it is absolutely necessary, imposed or if they think that the current conventions have too many negative consequences. Thus, true, intrinsic will-power to change behavior is lacking among these participants. Instead, they have a rather opportunistic attitude, meaning that their behavior is mostly driven by circumstances, not by principles.

P: “If tomorrow an alternative is offered by people that make that call, than I will move along. But I am not the one calling the shots on this issue.” (Interview 26, editor, Medical Sciences)

In addition, pragmatism often outweighs the perceived urgency to change. Participants argue they ‘just want to do their jobs’ and consider the practical consequences mainly in their actions. This attitude creates a certain degree of inertia. Although participants claim they are willing to change their behavior, this would contain much more than ‘doing their jobs, and thus, in the end, the NHST-debate is subject to ‘coffee talk’. People are open to discussion, but when it comes to taking action (and motivating others to do so), no one takes action.

P: “The endless analysis of your data to get something with a p-value less than 0.05… There are people that are more critical about that, and there are people that are less critical. But that is a subject for during the coffee break.” (Interview 18, professor, editor, Medical Sciences)

The goal of our study was to acquire in-depth insight into reasons why so many stakeholders from the scientific system keep using NHST as the default method to draw conclusions, despite its many well-documented drawbacks. Furthermore, we wanted to gain insight into the reasons for their reluctance to apply alternative methods. Using a theoretical framework [ 20 , 21 ], several interrelated facilitators and barriers associated with the use of NHST and alternative methods were identified. The identified factors are subsumed under three main themes: the scientific climate, the scientific duty and reactivity. The scientific climate is dominated by conventions, behavioral rules, and beliefs, of which the use of NHST and p-values is part. At the same time, stakeholders feel they have a (moral or professional) duty. For many participants, these two sides of the same coin are incompatible, leading to internal conflicts. There is a discrepancy between what participants want and what they do . As a result of these factors, the majority feels dependent on others and have thereby become reactive. Most participants are not inclined to take responsibility themselves but await action and initiatives from others. This may explain why NHST is still the standard and used by almost everyone involved.

The current study is closely related to the longstanding debate regarding NHST which recently increased to a level not seen before. In 2015, the editors of the journal ‘Basic and Applied Social Psychology’ (BASP) prohibited the use of NHST (and p-values and confidence intervals) [ 30 ]. Subsequently, in 2016, the American Statistical Association published the so-called ‘Statement on p-values’ in the American Statistician. This statement consists of critical standpoints regarding the use of NHST and p-values and warns against the abuse of the procedure. In 2019, the American Statistician devoted an entire edition to the implementation of reforms regarding the use of NHST; in more than forty articles, scientists debated statistical significance, advocated to embrace uncertainty, and suggested alternatives such as the use of s-values, False Positive Risks, reporting results as effect sizes and confidence intervals and more holistic approaches to p-values and outcome measures [ 31 ]. In addition, in the same year, several articles appeared in which an appeal was made to stop using statistical significance testing [ 32 , 33 ]. A number of counter-reactions were published [ 34 – 36 ], stating (i.e.) that banning statistical significance and, with that, abandoning clear rules for statistical analyses may create new problems with regard to statistical interpretation, study interpretations and objectivity. Also, some methodologists expressed the view that under certain circumstances the use of NHST and p-values is not problematic and can in fact provide useful answers [ 37 ]. Until recently, the NHST-debate was limited to mainly methodologists and statisticians. However, a growing number of scientists are getting involved in this lively debate and believe that a paradigm shift is desirable or even necessary.

The aforementioned publications have constructively contributed to this debate. In fact, since the publication of the special edition of the American Statistician, numerous scientific journals published editorials or revised, to a greater or lesser extent, their author guidelines [ 38 – 45 ]. Furthermore, following the American Statistical Association (ASA), the National Institute of Statistical Sciences (NISS) in the United States has also taken up the reform issue. However, real changes are still barely visible. It takes a long time before these kinds of initiatives translate into behavioral changes, and the widespread adoption by most of the scientific community is still far from accomplished. Debate alone will not lead to real changes, and therefore, our efforts to elucidate behavioral barriers and facilitators could provide a framework for potential effective initiatives that could be taken to reduce the default use of NHST. In fact, the debate could counteract behavioral change. If there is no consensus among statisticians and methodologists (the innovators), changing behavior cannot be expected from stakeholders with less statistical and methodological expertise. In other words, without agreement among innovators, early adopters might be reluctant to adopt the innovation.

Research has recently been conducted to explore the potential of behavioral change to improve Open Science behaviors. The adoption of open science behavior has increased in the last years, but uptake has been slow, due to firm barriers such as a lack of awareness about the subject, concerns about constrainment of the creative process, worries about being “scooped” and holding on to existing working practices [ 46 ]. The development regarding open science practices and the parallels these lines of research shows with the current study, might be of benefit to subserve behavioral change regarding the use of statistical methods.

The described obstacles to change behavior are related to features of both the ‘innovative idea’ and the potential adopters of the idea. First, there are characteristics of ‘the innovation’ that form barriers. The first barrier is the complexity of the innovation: most participants perceive alternative methods as difficult to understand and to use. A second barrier concerns the feasibility of trying the innovation; most people do not feel flexible about trying out or experimenting with the new idea. There is a lack of time and monetary resources to get acquainted with alternative methods (for example, by following a course). Also, the possible negative consequences of the use of alternatives (lower publications chances, the chance that the statistical method and message is too complicated for one’s readership) is holding people back from experimenting with these alternatives. And lastly, it is unclear for most participants what the visibility of the results of the new idea are. Up until now, the debate has mainly taken place among a small group of statisticians and methodologists. Many researchers are still not aware of the NHST debate and the idea to shift away from NHST and use alternative methods instead. Therefore, the question is how easily the benefits of the innovation can be made visible for a larger part of the scientific community. Thus, our study shows that, although the compatibility of the innovation is largely consistent with existing values (participants are critical about (the use of) NHST and the p-value and believe that there are better alternatives to NHST), important attributes of the innovative idea negatively affect the rate of adoption and consequently the diffusion of the innovation.

Due to the barriers mentioned above, most stakeholders do not have the intention to change their behavior and adopt the innovative idea. From the theory of planned behavior [ 21 ], it is known that behavioral intentions directly relate to performances of behaviors. The strength of the intention is shaped by attitudes, subjective norms, and perceived power. If people evaluate the suggested behavior as positive (attitude), and if they think others want them to perform the behavior (subjective norm), this leads to a stronger intention to perform that behavior. When an individual also perceives they have enough control over the behavior, they are likely to perform it. Although most participants have a positive attitude towards the behavior, or the innovative idea at stake, many participants think that others in their working environment believe that they should not perform the behavior—i.e., they do not approve of the use of alternative methods (social normative pressure). This is expressed, for example, in lower publication chances, negative judgements by supervisors or failing the requirements that are imposed by funding agencies. Thus, the perception about a particular behavior—the use of alternative methods—is negatively influenced by the (perceived) judgment of others. Moreover, we found that many participants have a low self-efficacy, meaning that there is a perceived lack of behavioral control, i.e., their perceived ability to engage in the behavior at issue is low. Also, participants feel a lack of authority (in the sense of knowledge and skills, but also power) to initiate behavioral change. The existing subjective norms and perceived behavioral control, and the negative attitudes towards performing the behavior, lead to a lower behavioral intention, and, ultimately, a lower chance of the performance of the actual behavior.

Several participants mentioned there is a need for people of stature (belonging to the group of early adopters) to take the lead and break down perceived barriers. Early adopters serve as role models and have opinion leadership, and form the next group (after the innovators, in this case statisticians and methodologists) to adopt an innovative idea [ 20 ] ( Fig 2 ). If early adopters would stand up, conveying a positive attitude towards the innovation, breaking down the described perceived barriers and facilitating the use of alternatives (for example by adjusting policy, guidelines and educational programs and making available financial resources for further training), this could positively affect the perceived social norms and self-efficacy of the early and late majority and ultimately laggards, which could ultimately lead to behavioral change among all stakeholders within the scientific community.

A strength of our study is that it is the first empirical study on views on the use of NHST, its alternatives and reasons for the prevailing use of NHST. Another strength is the method of coding which corresponds to the thematic approach from Braun & Clarke [ 47 ], which allows the researcher to move beyond just categorizing and coding the data, but also analyze how the codes are related to each other [ 47 ]. It provides a rich description of what is studied, linked to theory, but also generating new hypotheses. Moreover, two independent researchers coded all transcripts, which adds to the credibility of the study. All findings and the coding scheme were discussed by the two researchers, until consensus was reached. Also, interview results were further explored, enriched and validated by means of (mixed) focus groups. Important themes that emanated from the interviews, such as interdependency, perceptions on the scientific duty, perceived disadvantages of alternatives or the consequences of the current scientific climate, served as starting points and main subjects of the focus groups. This set-up provided more data, and more insight about the data and validation of the data. Lastly, the use of a theoretical framework [ 20 , 21 ] to develop the topic list, guide the interviews and focus groups, and guide their analysis is a strength as it provides structure to the analysis and substantiation of the results.

A limitation of this study is its sampling method. By using the network of members of the project group, and the fact that a relatively high proportion of those invited to participate refused because they thought they knew too little about the subject to be able to contribute, our sample was biased towards participants that are (somewhat) aware of the NHST debate. Our sample may also consist of people that are relatively critical towards the use of NHST, compared to the total population of researchers. It was not easy to include participants who were indifferent about or who were pro-NHST, as those were presumably less willing to make time and participate in this study. Even in our sample we found that the majority of our participants solely used NHST and perceived it as difficult if not impossible to change their behavior. These perceptions are thus probably even stronger in the target population. Another limitation, that is inherent to qualitative research, is the risk of interviewer bias. Respondents are unable, unwilling, or afraid to answer questions in good conscience, and instead provide socially desirable answers. In the context of our research, people are aware that, especially as a scientist, it does not look good to be conservative, complacent, or ignorant, or not to be open to innovation and new ideas. Therefore, some participants might have given a too favorable view of themselves. The interviewer bias can also take the other direction when values and expectations of the interviewer consciously or unconsciously influence the answers of the respondents. Although we have tried to be as neutral and objective as possible in asking questions and interpreting answers, we cannot rule out the chance that our views and opinions on the use of NHST have at times steered the respondents somewhat, potentially leading to the foregoing desirable answers.

Generalizability is a topic that is often debated in qualitative research methodology. Many researchers do not consider generalizability the purpose of qualitative research, but rather finding in-depth insights and explanations. However, this is an unjustified simplification, as generalizing of findings from qualitative research is possible. Three types of generalization in qualitative research are described: representational generalization (whether what is found in a sample can be generalized to the parent population of the sample), inferential generalization (whether findings from the study can be generalized to other settings), and theoretical generalization (where one draws theoretical statements from the findings of the study for more general application) [ 48 ]. The extent to which our results are generalizable is uncertain, as we used a theoretical sampling method, and our study was conducted exclusively in the Netherlands. We expect that the generic themes (reactivity, the scientific duty and the scientific climate) are applicable to academia in many countries across the world (inferential generalization). However, some elements, such as the Dutch educational system, will differ to a more or lesser extent from other countries (and thus can only be representationally generalized). In the Netherlands there is, for example, only one educational route after secondary school that has an academic orientation (scientific education, equivalent to the US university level education). This route consists of a bachelor’s program (typically 3 years), and a master’s program (typically 1, 2 or 3 years). Not every study program contains (compulsory) statistical courses, and statistical courses differ in depth and difficulty levels depending on the study program. Thus, not all the results will hold for other parts of the world, and further investigation is required.

Our findings demonstrate how perceived barriers to shift away from NHST set a high threshold for behavioral change and create a circle of interdependency. Behavioral change is a complex process. As ‘the stronger the intention to engage in a behavior, the more likely should be its performance’[ 21 ], further research on this subject should focus on how to influence the intention of behavior; i.e. which perceived barriers for the use of alternatives are most promising to break down in order to increase the intention for behavioral change. The present study shows that negative normative beliefs and a lack of perceived behavioral control regarding the innovation among individuals in the scientific system is a substantial problem. When social norms change in favor of the innovation, and control over the behavior increases, then the behavioral intention becomes a sufficient predictor of behavior [ 49 ]. An important follow-up question will therefore be: how can people be enthused and empowered, to ultimately take up the use of alternative methods instead of NHST? Answering this question can, in the long run, lead to the diffusion of the innovation through the scientific system as a whole.

NHST has been the leading paradigm for many decades and is deeply rooted in our science system, despite longstanding criticism. The aim of this study was to gain insight as to why we continue to use NHST. Our findings have demonstrated how perceived barriers to make a shift away from NHST set a high threshold for actual behavioral change and create a circle of interdependency between stakeholders in the scientific system. Consequently, people find themselves in a state of reactivity, which limits behavioral change with respect to the use of NHST. The next step would be to get more insight into ways to effectively remove barriers and thereby increase the intention to take a step back from NHST. A paradigm shift within a couple of years is not realistic. However, we believe that by taking small steps, one at a time, it is possible to decrease the scientific community’s strong dependence on NHST and p-values.

Supporting information

S1 appendix, acknowledgments.

The authors are grateful to Anja de Kruif for her contribution to the design of the study and for moderating one of the focus groups.

Funding Statement

This research was funded by the NWO (Nederlandse Organisatie voor Wetenschappelijk Onderzoek; Dutch Organization for Scientific Research) ( https://www.nwo.nl/ ) The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

Data Availability

Grad Coach

What Is A Research (Scientific) Hypothesis? A plain-language explainer + examples

By:  Derek Jansen (MBA)  | Reviewed By: Dr Eunice Rautenbach | June 2020

If you’re new to the world of research, or it’s your first time writing a dissertation or thesis, you’re probably noticing that the words “research hypothesis” and “scientific hypothesis” are used quite a bit, and you’re wondering what they mean in a research context .

“Hypothesis” is one of those words that people use loosely, thinking they understand what it means. However, it has a very specific meaning within academic research. So, it’s important to understand the exact meaning before you start hypothesizing. 

Research Hypothesis 101

  • What is a hypothesis ?
  • What is a research hypothesis (scientific hypothesis)?
  • Requirements for a research hypothesis
  • Definition of a research hypothesis
  • The null hypothesis

What is a hypothesis?

Let’s start with the general definition of a hypothesis (not a research hypothesis or scientific hypothesis), according to the Cambridge Dictionary:

Hypothesis: an idea or explanation for something that is based on known facts but has not yet been proved.

In other words, it’s a statement that provides an explanation for why or how something works, based on facts (or some reasonable assumptions), but that has not yet been specifically tested . For example, a hypothesis might look something like this:

Hypothesis: sleep impacts academic performance.

This statement predicts that academic performance will be influenced by the amount and/or quality of sleep a student engages in – sounds reasonable, right? It’s based on reasonable assumptions , underpinned by what we currently know about sleep and health (from the existing literature). So, loosely speaking, we could call it a hypothesis, at least by the dictionary definition.

But that’s not good enough…

Unfortunately, that’s not quite sophisticated enough to describe a research hypothesis (also sometimes called a scientific hypothesis), and it wouldn’t be acceptable in a dissertation, thesis or research paper . In the world of academic research, a statement needs a few more criteria to constitute a true research hypothesis .

What is a research hypothesis?

A research hypothesis (also called a scientific hypothesis) is a statement about the expected outcome of a study (for example, a dissertation or thesis). To constitute a quality hypothesis, the statement needs to have three attributes – specificity , clarity and testability .

Let’s take a look at these more closely.

Need a helping hand?

alternative hypothesis in quantitative research

Hypothesis Essential #1: Specificity & Clarity

A good research hypothesis needs to be extremely clear and articulate about both what’ s being assessed (who or what variables are involved ) and the expected outcome (for example, a difference between groups, a relationship between variables, etc.).

Let’s stick with our sleepy students example and look at how this statement could be more specific and clear.

Hypothesis: Students who sleep at least 8 hours per night will, on average, achieve higher grades in standardised tests than students who sleep less than 8 hours a night.

As you can see, the statement is very specific as it identifies the variables involved (sleep hours and test grades), the parties involved (two groups of students), as well as the predicted relationship type (a positive relationship). There’s no ambiguity or uncertainty about who or what is involved in the statement, and the expected outcome is clear.

Contrast that to the original hypothesis we looked at – “Sleep impacts academic performance” – and you can see the difference. “Sleep” and “academic performance” are both comparatively vague , and there’s no indication of what the expected relationship direction is (more sleep or less sleep). As you can see, specificity and clarity are key.

A good research hypothesis needs to be very clear about what’s being assessed and very specific about the expected outcome.

Hypothesis Essential #2: Testability (Provability)

A statement must be testable to qualify as a research hypothesis. In other words, there needs to be a way to prove (or disprove) the statement. If it’s not testable, it’s not a hypothesis – simple as that.

For example, consider the hypothesis we mentioned earlier:

Hypothesis: Students who sleep at least 8 hours per night will, on average, achieve higher grades in standardised tests than students who sleep less than 8 hours a night.  

We could test this statement by undertaking a quantitative study involving two groups of students, one that gets 8 or more hours of sleep per night for a fixed period, and one that gets less. We could then compare the standardised test results for both groups to see if there’s a statistically significant difference. 

Again, if you compare this to the original hypothesis we looked at – “Sleep impacts academic performance” – you can see that it would be quite difficult to test that statement, primarily because it isn’t specific enough. How much sleep? By who? What type of academic performance?

So, remember the mantra – if you can’t test it, it’s not a hypothesis 🙂

A good research hypothesis must be testable. In other words, you must able to collect observable data in a scientifically rigorous fashion to test it.

Defining A Research Hypothesis

You’re still with us? Great! Let’s recap and pin down a clear definition of a hypothesis.

A research hypothesis (or scientific hypothesis) is a statement about an expected relationship between variables, or explanation of an occurrence, that is clear, specific and testable.

So, when you write up hypotheses for your dissertation or thesis, make sure that they meet all these criteria. If you do, you’ll not only have rock-solid hypotheses but you’ll also ensure a clear focus for your entire research project.

What about the null hypothesis?

You may have also heard the terms null hypothesis , alternative hypothesis, or H-zero thrown around. At a simple level, the null hypothesis is the counter-proposal to the original hypothesis.

For example, if the hypothesis predicts that there is a relationship between two variables (for example, sleep and academic performance), the null hypothesis would predict that there is no relationship between those variables.

At a more technical level, the null hypothesis proposes that no statistical significance exists in a set of given observations and that any differences are due to chance alone.

And there you have it – hypotheses in a nutshell. 

If you have any questions, be sure to leave a comment below and we’ll do our best to help you. If you need hands-on help developing and testing your hypotheses, consider our private coaching service , where we hold your hand through the research journey.

alternative hypothesis in quantitative research

Psst... there’s more!

This post was based on one of our popular Research Bootcamps . If you're working on a research project, you'll definitely want to check this out ...

You Might Also Like:

Research limitations vs delimitations

16 Comments

Lynnet Chikwaikwai

Very useful information. I benefit more from getting more information in this regard.

Dr. WuodArek

Very great insight,educative and informative. Please give meet deep critics on many research data of public international Law like human rights, environment, natural resources, law of the sea etc

Afshin

In a book I read a distinction is made between null, research, and alternative hypothesis. As far as I understand, alternative and research hypotheses are the same. Can you please elaborate? Best Afshin

GANDI Benjamin

This is a self explanatory, easy going site. I will recommend this to my friends and colleagues.

Lucile Dossou-Yovo

Very good definition. How can I cite your definition in my thesis? Thank you. Is nul hypothesis compulsory in a research?

Pereria

It’s a counter-proposal to be proven as a rejection

Egya Salihu

Please what is the difference between alternate hypothesis and research hypothesis?

Mulugeta Tefera

It is a very good explanation. However, it limits hypotheses to statistically tasteable ideas. What about for qualitative researches or other researches that involve quantitative data that don’t need statistical tests?

Derek Jansen

In qualitative research, one typically uses propositions, not hypotheses.

Samia

could you please elaborate it more

Patricia Nyawir

I’ve benefited greatly from these notes, thank you.

Hopeson Khondiwa

This is very helpful

Dr. Andarge

well articulated ideas are presented here, thank you for being reliable sources of information

TAUNO

Excellent. Thanks for being clear and sound about the research methodology and hypothesis (quantitative research)

I have only a simple question regarding the null hypothesis. – Is the null hypothesis (Ho) known as the reversible hypothesis of the alternative hypothesis (H1? – How to test it in academic research?

Tesfaye Negesa Urge

this is very important note help me much more

Trackbacks/Pingbacks

  • What Is Research Methodology? Simple Definition (With Examples) - Grad Coach - […] Contrasted to this, a quantitative methodology is typically used when the research aims and objectives are confirmatory in nature. For example,…

Submit a Comment Cancel reply

Your email address will not be published. Required fields are marked *

Save my name, email, and website in this browser for the next time I comment.

  • Print Friendly

IMAGES

  1. Best Example of How to Write a Hypothesis 2024

    alternative hypothesis in quantitative research

  2. Null Hypothesis and Alternative Hypothesis

    alternative hypothesis in quantitative research

  3. Research Hypothesis Generator

    alternative hypothesis in quantitative research

  4. Hypothesis Meaning In Quantitative Research

    alternative hypothesis in quantitative research

  5. Null and Alternative Hypothesis: Symbolic Notation

    alternative hypothesis in quantitative research

  6. below shows the Null Hypothesis versus the Alternative Hypothesis with

    alternative hypothesis in quantitative research

VIDEO

  1. Intro to Statistics Basic Concepts and Research Techniques

  2. Group 1

  3. Research Methods

  4. Null Hypothesis vs Alternate Hypothesis

  5. Research understanding

  6. Null Hypothesis vs Alternate Hypothesis

COMMENTS

  1. Null & Alternative Hypotheses

    The null and alternative hypotheses offer competing answers to your research question. When the research question asks "Does the independent variable affect the dependent variable?": The null hypothesis ( H0) answers "No, there's no effect in the population.". The alternative hypothesis ( Ha) answers "Yes, there is an effect in the ...

  2. Null and Alternative Hypotheses

    The null and alternative hypotheses offer competing answers to your research question. When the research question asks "Does the independent variable affect the dependent variable?", the null hypothesis (H 0) answers "No, there's no effect in the population.". On the other hand, the alternative hypothesis (H A) answers "Yes, there ...

  3. 9.1 Null and Alternative Hypotheses

    The actual test begins by considering two hypotheses.They are called the null hypothesis and the alternative hypothesis.These hypotheses contain opposing viewpoints. H 0, the —null hypothesis: a statement of no difference between sample means or proportions or no difference between a sample mean or proportion and a population mean or proportion. In other words, the difference equals 0.

  4. Null & Alternative Hypotheses

    In research, there are two types of hypotheses: null and alternative. They work as a complementary pair, each stating that the other is wrong. Null Hypothesis (H0) - This can be thought of as the implied hypothesis. "Null" meaning "nothing.". This hypothesis states that there is no difference between groups or no relationship between ...

  5. A Practical Guide to Writing Quantitative and Qualitative Research

    INTRODUCTION. Scientific research is usually initiated by posing evidenced-based research questions which are then explicitly restated as hypotheses.1,2 The hypotheses provide directions to guide the study, solutions, explanations, and expected results.3,4 Both research questions and hypotheses are essentially formulated based on conventional theories and real-world processes, which allow the ...

  6. An Introduction to Statistics: Understanding Hypothesis Testing and

    HYPOTHESIS TESTING. A clinical trial begins with an assumption or belief, and then proceeds to either prove or disprove this assumption. In statistical terms, this belief or assumption is known as a hypothesis. Counterintuitively, what the researcher believes in (or is trying to prove) is called the "alternate" hypothesis, and the opposite ...

  7. Null and Alternative Hypotheses

    The actual test begins by considering two hypotheses.They are called the null hypothesis and the alternative hypothesis.These hypotheses contain opposing viewpoints. H 0: The null hypothesis: It is a statement about the population that either is believed to be true or is used to put forth an argument unless it can be shown to be incorrect beyond a reasonable doubt.

  8. How to Write a Strong Hypothesis

    6. Write a null hypothesis. If your research involves statistical hypothesis testing, you will also have to write a null hypothesis. The null hypothesis is the default position that there is no association between the variables. The null hypothesis is written as H 0, while the alternative hypothesis is H 1 or H a.

  9. Null and Alternative Hypotheses

    Converting research questions to hypothesis is a simple task. Take the questions and make it a positive statement that says a relationship exists (correlation studies) or a difference exists between the groups (experiment study) and you have the alternative hypothesis. Write the statement such that a relationship does not exist or a difference ...

  10. 9.2: Null and Alternative Hypotheses

    Review. In a hypothesis test, sample data is evaluated in order to arrive at a decision about some type of claim.If certain conditions about the sample are satisfied, then the claim can be evaluated for a population. In a hypothesis test, we: Evaluate the null hypothesis, typically denoted with \(H_{0}\).The null is not rejected unless the hypothesis test shows otherwise.

  11. Quantitative Research

    Hypothesis testing (sometimes called significance testing) in quantitative research is conducted on either the "null" or the "alternative" hypothesis. 8.1 The Null Hypothesis The null hypothesis is a statement that assumes the problem being investigated is absent or has no effect (i.e., the difference between means equals zero).

  12. Research Questions & Hypotheses

    The primary research question should originate from the hypothesis, not the data, and be established before starting the study. Formulating the research question and hypothesis from existing data (e.g., a database) can lead to multiple statistical comparisons and potentially spurious findings due to chance.

  13. PDF Research Questions and Hypotheses

    Research Questions and Hypotheses. I. nvestigators place signposts to carry the reader through a plan for a study. The first signpost is the purpose statement, which establishes the central direction for the study. From the broad, general purpose state- ment, the researcher narrows the focus to specific questions to be answered or predictions ...

  14. 10.1

    The alternative hypothesis is typically the research hypothesis of interest. Here are some examples. Example 10.2: Hypotheses with One Sample of One Categorical Variable Section . About 10% of the human population is left-handed. Suppose a researcher at Penn State speculates that students in the College of Arts and Architecture are more likely ...

  15. Formulating Hypotheses for Different Study Designs

    Formulating Hypotheses for Different Study Designs. Generating a testable working hypothesis is the first step towards conducting original research. Such research may prove or disprove the proposed hypothesis. Case reports, case series, online surveys and other observational studies, clinical trials, and narrative reviews help to generate ...

  16. Hypothesis Testing

    Hypothesis Testing. When you conduct a piece of quantitative research, you are inevitably attempting to answer a research question or hypothesis that you have set. One method of evaluating this research question is via a process called hypothesis testing, which is sometimes also referred to as significance testing. Since there are many facets ...

  17. Constructing Hypotheses in Quantitative Research

    Hypotheses are the testable statements linked to your research question. Hypotheses bridge the gap from the general question you intend to investigate (i.e., the research question) to concise statements of what you hypothesize the connection between your variables to be. For example, if we were studying the influence of mentoring relationships ...

  18. Examples of null and alternative hypotheses

    It is the opposite of your research hypothesis. The alternative hypothesis--that is, the research hypothesis--is the idea, phenomenon, observation that you want to prove. If you suspect that girls take longer to get ready for school than boys, then: Alternative: girls time > boys time. Null: girls time <= boys time.

  19. Null hypothesis and Alternative Hypothesis

    The null hypothesis and alternative hypothesis are required to be fragmented properly before the data collection and interpretation phase in the research. Well fragmented hypotheses indicate that the researcher has adequate knowledge in that particular area and is thus able to take the investigation further because they can use a much more ...

  20. How to Write a Null and Alternative Hypothesis: A Guide with Examples

    Alternatively, researchers can change the question into a positive statement that includes a relationship that exists between the variables. In turn, this latter statement becomes the alternative hypothesis and is symbolized as H1. Hence, some of the examples of research questions and hull and alternative hypotheses are as follows: 1.

  21. PDF DEVELOPING HYPOTHESIS AND RESEARCH QUESTIONS

    "A hypothesis is a conjectural statement of the relation between two or more variables". (Kerlinger, 1956) "Hypothesis is a formal statement that presents the expected relationship between an independent and dependent variable."(Creswell, 1994) "A research question is essentially a hypothesis asked in the form of a question."

  22. Why we habitually engage in null-hypothesis significance testing: A

    At this point, researchers commonly reject the null hypothesis and accept the alternative hypothesis [ 2 ]. Assessing statistical significance by means of contrasting the data with the null hypothesis is called Null Hypothesis Significance Testing (NHST). NHST is the best known and most widely used statistical procedure for making inferences ...

  23. What Is A Research Hypothesis? A Simple Definition

    A research hypothesis (also called a scientific hypothesis) is a statement about the expected outcome of a study (for example, a dissertation or thesis). To constitute a quality hypothesis, the statement needs to have three attributes - specificity, clarity and testability. Let's take a look at these more closely.