An official website of the United States government
Official websites use .gov A .gov website belongs to an official government organization in the United States.
Secure .gov websites use HTTPS A lock ( Lock Locked padlock icon ) or https:// means you've safely connected to the .gov website. Share sensitive information only on official, secure websites.
- Publications
- Account settings
- Advanced Search
- Journal List
The Economic Importance of Financial Literacy: Theory and Evidence
Annamaria lusardi, olivia s mitchell.
- Author information
- Copyright and License information
This paper undertakes an assessment of a rapidly growing body of economic research on financial literacy. We start with an overview of theoretical research which casts financial knowledge as a form of investment in human capital. Endogenizing financial knowledge has important implications for welfare as well as policies intended to enhance levels of financial knowledge in the larger population. Next, we draw on recent surveys to establish how much (or how little) people know and identify the least financially savvy population subgroups. This is followed by an examination of the impact of financial literacy on economic decision-making in the United States and elsewhere. While the literature is still young, conclusions may be drawn about the effects and consequences of financial illiteracy and what works to remedy these gaps. A final section offers thoughts on what remains to be learned if researchers are to better inform theoretical and empirical models as well as public policy.
1. Introduction
Financial markets around the world have become increasingly accessible to the ‘small investor,’ as new products and financial services grow widespread. At the onset of the recent financial crisis, consumer credit and mortgage borrowing had burgeoned. People who had credit cards or subprime mortgages were in the historically unusual position of being able to decide how much they wanted to borrow. Alternative financial services, including payday loans, pawn shops, auto title loans, tax refund loans, and rent-to-own shops have also become widespread. 1 At the same time, changes in the pension landscape are increasingly thrusting responsibility for saving, investing, and decumulating wealth onto workers and retirees, whereas in the past, older workers relied mainly on Social Security and employer-sponsored defined benefit (DB) pension plans in retirement. Today, by contrast, Baby Boomers mainly have defined contribution (DC) plans and Individual Retirement Accounts (IRAs) during their working years. This trend toward disintermediation increasingly is requiring people to decide how much to save and where to invest, and during retirement, to take on responsibility for careful decumulation so as not to outlive their assets while meeting their needs. 2
Despite the rapid spread of such financially complex products to the retail marketplace, including student loans, mortgages, credit cards, pension accounts, and annuities, many of these have proven to be difficult for financially unsophisticated investors to master. 3 Therefore, while these developments have their advantages, they also impose on households a much greater responsibility to borrow, save, invest, and decumulate their assets sensibly by permitting tailored financial contracts and more people to access credit. Accordingly, one goal of this paper is to offer an assessment of how well-equipped today’s households are to make these complex financial decisions. Specifically we focus on financial literacy , by which we mean peoples’ ability to process economic information and make informed decisions about financial planning, wealth accumulation, debt, and pensions. In what follows, we outline recent theoretical research modeling how financial knowledge can be cast as a type of investment in human capital. In this framework, those who build financial savvy can earn above-average expected returns on their investments, yet there will still be some optimal level of financial ignorance. Endogenizing financial knowledge has important implications for welfare, and this perspective also offers insights into programs intended to enhance levels of financial knowledge in the larger population.
Another of our goals is to assess the effects of financial literacy on important economic behaviors. We do so by drawing on evidence about what people know and which groups are the least financially literate. Moreover, the literature allows us to tease out the impact of financial literacy on economic decision-making in the United States and abroad, along with the costs of financial ignorance. Because this is a new area of economic research, we conclude with thoughts on policies to help fill these gaps; we focus on what remains to be learned to better inform theoretical/empirical models and public policy.
2. A Theoretical Framework for Financial Literacy
The conventional microeconomic approach to saving and consumption decisions posits that a fully rational and well-informed individual will consume less than his income in times of high earnings, thus saving to support consumption when income falls (e.g. after retirement). Starting with Modigliani and Brumberg (1954) and Friedman (1957) , the consumer is posited to arrange his optimal saving and decumulation patterns to smooth marginal utility over his lifetime. Many studies have shown how such a life cycle optimization process can be shaped by consumer preferences (e.g. risk aversion and discount rates), the economic environment (e.g. risky returns on investments and liquidity constraints), and social safety net benefits (e.g. the availability and generosity of welfare schemes and Social Security benefits), among other features. 4
These microeconomic models generally assume that individuals can formulate and execute saving and spend-down plans, which requires them to have the capacity to undertake complex economic calculations and to have expertise in dealing with financial markets. As we show below in detail, however, few people seem to have much financial knowledge. Moreover, acquiring such knowledge is likely to come at a cost. In the past, when retirement pensions were designed and implemented by governments, individual workers devote very little attention to their plan details. Today, by contrast, since saving, investment, and decumulation for retirement are occurring in an increasingly personalized pension environment, the gaps between modeling and reality are worth exploring, so as to better evaluate where the theory can be enriched, and how policy efforts can be better targeted.
Though there is a substantial theoretical and empirical body of work on the economics of education, 5 far less attention has been devoted to the question of how people acquire and deploy financial literacy. In the last few years, however, a few papers have begun to examine the decision to acquire financial literacy and to study the links between financial knowledge, saving, and investment behavior ( Delavande, Rohwedder, and Willis 2008 ; Jappelli and Padula 2013 ; Hsu 2011 ; and Lusardi, Michaud, and Mitchell 2013 ). 6 For instance, Delavande, Rohwedder, and Willis (2008) present a simple two-period model of saving and portfolio allocation across safe bonds and risky stocks, allowing for the acquisition of human capital in the form of financial knowledge ( à la Ben-Porath, 1967 , and Becker, 1975 ). That work posits that individuals will optimally elect to invest in financial knowledge to gain access to higher-return assets: this training helps them identify better-performing assets and/or hire financial advisers who can reduce investment expenses. Hsu (2011) uses a similar approach in an intra-household setting where husbands specialize in the acquisition of financial knowledge, while wives increase their acquisition of financial knowledge mostly when it becomes relevant (such as prior to the death of their spouses). Jappelli and Padula (2013) also consider a two-period model but additionally sketch a multi-period life cycle model with financial literacy endogenously determined. They predict that financial literacy and wealth will be strongly correlated over the life cycle, with both rising until retirement and falling thereafter. They also suggest that in countries with generous Social Security benefits, there will be fewer incentives to save and accumulate wealth and, in turn, less reason to invest in financial literacy.
Each of these studies represents a useful theoretical advance, yet none incorporates key features now standard in theoretical models of saving – namely borrowing constraints, mortality risk, demographic factors, stock market returns, and earnings and health shocks. These shortcomings are rectified in recent work by Lusardi, Michaud, and Mitchell (2011 , 2013 ), which calibrates and simulates a multi-period dynamic life cycle model where individuals not only select capital market investments but also undertake investments in financial knowledge. This extension is important in that it permits the researchers to examine model implications for wealth inequality and welfare. Two distinct investment technologies are considered: the first is a simple technology which pays a fixed low rate of return each period ( R ¯ = 1 + r ¯ ) , similar to a bank account, while the second is a more sophisticated technology providing the consumer access to a higher stochastic expected return, R ∼ ( f t ) , which depends on his accumulated level of financial knowledge. Each period, the stock of knowledge is related to what the individual had in the previous period minus a depreciation factor: thus f t +1 = δ f t + i t , where δ represents knowledge depreciation (due to obsolescence or decay), and gross investment in knowledge is indicated with i t . The stochastic return from the sophisticated technology follows the process R ∼ ( f t + 1 ) = R ¯ + r ( f t + 1 ) + σ ε ε t + 1 (where ε t is a N(0,1) iid shock and σ ε refers to the standard deviation of returns on the sophisticated technology). To access this higher expected return, the consumer must pay both a direct cost (c), and a time and money cost ( π ) to build up knowledge. 7
Prior to retirement, the individual earns risky labor income ( y ) from which he can consume or invest so as to raise his return (R) on saving (s) by investing in the sophisticated technology. After retirement, the individual receives Social Security benefits which are a percentage of pre-retirement income. 8 Additional sources of uncertainty include stock returns, medical costs, and longevity. Each period, therefore, the consumer’s decision variables are how much to invest in the capital market, consume ( c ), and whether to invest in financial knowledge.
Assuming a discount rate of β and η o , η y , and ε which refer, respectively, to shocks in medical expenditures, labor earnings, and rate of return, the problem takes the form of a series of Bellman equations with the following value function V d ( s t ) at each age as long as the individual is alive ( p e , t > 0):
The utility function is assumed to be strictly concave in consumption and scaled using the function u ( c t / n t ) where n t is an equivalence scale capturing family size which changes predictably over the life cycle; and by education, subscripted by e . End-of-period assets ( a t +1 ) are equal to labor earnings plus the returns on the previous period’s saving plus transfer income (tr) , minus consumption and costs of investment in knowledge (as long as investments are positive; i.e., κ > 0. Accordingly, a t + 1 = R ∼ κ ( f t + 1 ) ( a t + y e , t + t r t − c t − π ( i t ) − c d I ( κ t > 0 ) ) 9 .
After calibrating the model using plausible parameter values, the authors then solve the value functions for consumers with low/medium/high educational levels by backward recursion. 10 Given paths of optimal consumption, knowledge investment, and participation in the stock market, they then simulate 5,000 life cycles allowing for return, income, and medical expense shocks. 11
Several key predictions emerge from this study. First, endogenously-determined optimal paths for financial knowledge are hump-shaped over the life cycle. Second, consumers invest in financial knowledge to the point where their marginal time and money costs of doing so are equated to their marginal benefits; of course, this optimum will depend on the cost function for financial knowledge acquisition. Third, knowledge profiles differ across educational groups because of peoples’ different life cycle income profiles.
Importantly, this model also predicts that inequality in wealth and financial knowledge will arise endogenously without having to rely on assumed cross-sectional differences in preferences or other major changes to the theoretical setup. 12 Moreover, differences in wealth across education groups also arise endogenously; that is, some population subgroups optimally have low financial literacy, particularly those anticipating substantial safety net income in old age. Finally, the model implies that financial education programs should not be expected to produce large behavioral changes for the least educated, since it may not be worthwhile for the least educated to incur knowledge investment costs given that their consumption needs are better insured by transfer programs. 13 This prediction is consistent with Jappelli and Padula’s (2013) suggestion that less financially informed individuals will be found in countries with more generous Social Security benefits (see also Jappelli 2010 ).
Despite the fact that some people will rationally choose to invest little or nothing in financial knowledge, the model predicts that it can still be socially optimal to raise financial knowledge for everyone early in life, for instance by mandating financial education in high school. This is because even if the least educated never invest again and let their knowledge endowment depreciate, they will still earn higher returns on their saving which generates a substantial welfare boost. For instance, providing pre-labor market financial knowledge to the least educated group improves their wellbeing by an amount equivalent to 82 percent of their initial wealth ( Lusardi, Michaud, and Mitchell 2011 ). The wealth equivalent value for college graduates is also estimated to be substantial, at 56 percent. These estimates are, of course, specific to the calibration, but the approach underscores that consumers would benefit from acquiring financial knowledge early in life even if they made no new investments thereafter.
In sum, a small but growing theoretical literature on financial literacy has made strides in recent years by endogenizing the process of financial knowledge acquisition, generating predictions that can be tested empirically, and offering a coherent way to evaluate policy options. Moreover, these models offer insights into how policymakers might enhance welfare by enhancing young workers’ endowment of financial knowledge. In the next section, we turn to a review of empirical evidence on financial literacy and how to measure it in practice. Subsequently, we analyze existing studies on how financial knowledge matters for economic behavior in the empirical realm.
3. Measuring Financial Literacy
Several fundamental concepts lie at the root of saving and investment decisions as modeled in the life cycle setting described in the previous section. Three such concepts are: (i) numeracy and capacity to do calculations related to interest rates , such as compound interest; (ii) understanding of inflation ; and (iii) understanding of risk diversification . Translating these into easily-measured financial literacy metrics is difficult, but Lusardi and Mitchell (2008 , 2011b , c ) have designed a standard set of questions around these ideas and implemented them in numerous surveys in the United States and abroad.
Four principles informed the design of these questions. The first is Simplicity : the questions should measure knowledge of the building blocks fundamental to decision-making in an intertemporal setting. The second is Relevance : the questions should relate to concepts pertinent to peoples’ day-to-day financial decisions over the life cycle; moreover, they must capture general rather than context-specific ideas. Third is Brevity : the number of questions must be kept short to secure widespread adoption; and fourth is Capacity to differentiate , meaning that questions should differentiate financial knowledge to permit comparisons across people. These criteria are met by the three financial literacy questions designed by Lusardi and Mitchell (2008 Lusardi and Mitchell (2011b ), worded as follows:
Suppose you had $100 in a savings account and the interest rate was 2% per year. After 5 years, how much do you think you would have in the account if you left the money to grow: [more than $102, exactly $102, less than $102? Do not know, refuse to answer.]
Imagine that the interest rate on your savings account was 1% per year and inflation was 2% per year. After 1 year, would you be able to buy: [more than, exactly the same as, or less than today with the money in this account? Do not know; refuse to answer.]
Do you think that the following statement is true or false? ‘Buying a single company stock usually provides a safer return than a stock mutual fund.’ [Do not know; refuse to answer.]
The first question measures numeracy or the capacity to do a simple calculation related to compounding of interest rates. The second question measures understanding of inflation, again in the context of a simple financial decision. The third question is a joint test of knowledge about ‘stocks’ and ‘stock mutual funds’ and of risk diversification, since the answer to this question depends on knowing what a stock is and that a mutual fund is composed of many stocks. As is clear from the theoretical models described earlier, many decisions about retirement savings must deal with financial markets. Accordingly, it is important to understand knowledge of the stock market as well as differentiate between levels of financial knowledge.
Naturally any given set of financial literacy measures can only proxy for what individuals need to know to optimize behavior in intertemporal models of financial decision-making. 14 Moreover, measurement error is a concern, as well as the possibility that answers might not measure ‘true’ financial knowledge. These issues have implications for empirical work on financial literacy, to be discussed below.
Empirical Evidence of Financial Literacy in the Adult Population
The three questions above were first administered to a representative sample of U.S. respondents age 50 and older, in a special module of the 2004 Health and Retirement Study (HRS). 15 Results, summarized in Table 1 , indicate that this older U.S. population is quite financially illiterate: only about half could answer the simple 2 percent calculation and knew about inflation, and only one third could answer all three questions correctly ( Lusardi and Mitchell 2011b ). This poor showing is notwithstanding the fact that people in this age group had made many financial decisions and engaged in numerous financial transactions over their lifetimes. Moreover, these respondents had experienced two or three periods of high inflation (depending on their age) and witnessed numerous economic and stock market shocks (including the demise of Enron), which should have provided them with information about investment risk. In fact, the question about risk is the one where respondents answered disproportionately with “Do not know.”
Financial Literacy Patterns in the United States
Source: Authors’ computations from HRS 2004 Planning Module
Note: DK = respondent indicated “don’t know.”
These same questions were added to several other U.S. surveys thereafter, including the 2007–2008 National Longitudinal Survey of Youth (NLSY) for young respondents (ages 23–28) ( Lusardi, Mitchell, and Curto 2010 ); the RAND American Life Panel (ALP) covering all ages ( Lusardi and Mitchell 2009 ); and the 2009 and 2012 National Financial Capability Study ( Lusardi and Mitchell 2011d ). 16 In each case, the findings underscore and extend the HRS results, in that for all groups, the level of financial literacy in the U.S. was found to be quite low.
Additional and more sophisticated concepts were then added to the financial literacy measures. For instance, the 2009 and 2012 National Financial Capability Survey included two items measuring sophisticated concepts such as asset pricing and understanding of mortgages/mortgage payments. Results revealed additional gaps in knowledge: for example, data from the 2009 wave show that only a small percentage of Americans (21%) knew about the inverse relationship between bond prices and interest rates ( Lusardi 2011 ). 17 A pass/fail set of 28 questions by Hilgert, Hogarth, and Beverly (2003) covered knowledge of credit, saving patterns, mortgages, and general financial management, and the authors concluded most people earned a failing score on these questions as well. 18 Lusardi, Mitchell and Curto (2012) also examine a set to questions measuring financial sophistication in addition to basic financial literacy and found that a large majority of older respondents are not financially sophisticated. Additional surveys have also examined financial knowledge in the context of debt. For example, Lusardi and Tufano (2009a , b ) examined ‘debt literacy’ regarding interest compounding and found that only one-third of respondents knew how long it would take for debt to double if one were to borrow at a 20 percent interest rate. This lack of knowledge confirms conclusions from Moore’s (2003) survey of Washington state residents, where she found that people frequently failed to understand interest compounding along with the terms and conditions of consumer loans and mortgages. Studies have also looked at different measures of “risk literacy” ( Lusardi, Schneider, and Tufano 2011 ). Knowledge of risk and risk diversification remains low even when the questions are formulated in alternative ways (see, Kimball and Shumway 2006 ; Yoong 2011 ; and Lusardi, Schneider, and Tufano 2011 ). In other words, all of these surveys confirm that most U.S. respondents are not financially literate.
Empirical Evidence of Financial Literacy among the Young
As noted above, it would be useful to know how well-informed people are at the start of their working lives. Several authors have measured high school students’ financial literacy using data from the Jump$tart Coalition for Personal Financial Literacy and the Council for Financial Education (CEE). Because those studies included a long list of questions, they provide a rather nuanced evaluation of what young people know when they enter the workforce. As we saw for their adult counterparts, most high school students in the U.S. receive a failing financial literacy grade ( Mandell 2008 ; Markow and Bagnaschi 2005 ). Similar findings are reported for college students ( Chen and Volpe 1998 ; and Shim, Barber, Card, Xiao, and Serido 2010 ).
International Evidence on Financial Literacy
The three questions mentioned earlier and that have been used in several surveys in the United States have also been used in several national surveys in other countries. Table 2 reports the findings from the twelve countries that have used these questions and where comparisons can be made for the total population. 19 For brevity, we only report the proportion of correct and do not know answers to each questions and for all questions.
Comparative Statistics on Responses to Financial Literacy Questions around the World
indicates questions that have slightly different wording than the baseline financial literacy questions enumerated in the text.
The table highlights a few key findings. First, few people across countries can correctly answer three basic financial literacy questions. In the U.S., only 30 percent can do so, with similar low percentages in countries having well-developed financial markets (Germany, the Netherlands, Japan, Australia and others), as well as in nations where financial markets are changing rapidly (Russia and Romania). In other words, low levels of financial literacy found in the U.S. are also prevalent elsewhere, rather than being specific to any given country or stage of economic development. Second, some of what adult respondents know is related to national historical experience. For example, Germans and Dutch are more likely to know the answer to the inflation question, whereas many fewer people do in Japan, a country that has experienced deflation. Countries that were planned economies in the past (such as Romania and Russia) displayed the lowest knowledge of inflation. Third, of the questions examined, risk diversification appears to be the concept that people have the most difficulty grasping. Virtually everywhere, a high share of people respond that they ‘do not know’ the answer to the risk diversification question. For instance, in the U.S., 34 percent of respondents state they do not know the answer to the risk diversification question; in Germany 32 percent and the Netherlands 33 percent do so; and even in the most risk-savvy country of Sweden and Switzerland, 18 and 13 percent respectively report they do not know the answer to the risk diversification question.
The Organisation for Economic Co-operation and Development (OECD) has been a pioneer in highlighting the lack of financial literacy across countries. For example, an OECD report in 2005 documented extensive financial illiteracy in Europe, Australia, and Japan, among others. 20 More recently, Atkinson and Messy (2011 Atkinson and Messy (2012) confirmed the patterns of financial illiteracy mentioned earlier in the text across 14 countries at different stages of development in four continents, using a harmonized set of financial literacy as in the three questions that were used in many countries. 21
The goal of evaluating student financial knowledge around the world among the young (high school students) has recently been taken up by the OECD’s Programme for International Student Assessment (PISA), 22 which in 2012 added a module on financial literacy to its review of proficiency in mathematics, science, and reading. Accordingly, 15-year olds around the world will be able to be compared with regard to their financial knowledge. In so doing, PISA has taken the position that financial literacy should be recognized as a skill essential for participation in today’s economy.
Objective versus Subjective Measures of Financial Literacy
Another interesting finding on financial literacy is that there is often a substantial mismatch between peoples’ self-assessed knowledge versus their actual knowledge , where the latter is measured by correct answers to the financial literacy questions posed. As one example, several surveys include questions asking people to indicate their self-assessed knowledge, as the following questions used in the United States and also in the Netherlands and Germany:
On a scale from 1 to 7, where 1 means very low and 7 means very high, how would you assess your overall financial knowledge?’
Even though actual financial literacy levels are low, respondents are generally rather confident of their financial knowledge and, overall, they tend to overestimate how much they know ( Table 3 ). For instance in the 2009 U.S. Financial Capability Study, 70 percent of respondents gave themselves score of 4 or higher (out of 7), but only 30 percent of the sample could answer the factual questions correctly ( Lusardi 2011 ). Similar findings were reported in other U.S. surveys and in Germany and the Netherlands ( Bucher-Koenen, Lusardi, Alessie and van Rooij 2012 ). One exception is Japan, where respondents gave themselves low grades in financial knowledge. In other words, though actual financial literacy is low, most people are unaware of their own shortcomings.
Comparative Statistics on Responses to Self-reported Financial Literacy
Note: This table reports respondents’ answers to the question: “On a scale from 1 to 7, where 1 means very low and 7 means very high, how would you assess your overall financial knowledge?” Note that the question posed in Lusardi and Mitchell (2009) is different and asks the following: “How would you assess your understanding of economics (on a 7-point scale;1 means very low and 7 means very high)?” In Japan, respondents were asked whether they think that they know a lot about finance on a 1–5 point scale ( Sekita 2011 ).
Financial Literacy and Framing
Peoples’ responses to survey questions cannot always be taken at face value, a point well-known to psychometricians and economic statisticians. One reason, as noted above, is that financial literacy may be measured with error, depending on the way questions are worded. To test this possibility, Lusardi and Mitchell (2009) and van Rooij, Lusardi, and Alessie (2011) randomly asked two groups of respondents the same risk question, but randomized their order of presentation. Thus half the group received format (a) and the other half format (b), as follows:
Buying a company stock usually provides a safer return than a stock mutual fund. True or false?
Buying a stock mutual fund usually provides a safer return than a company stock. True or false?
They found that people’s responses were, indeed, sensitive to how the question was worded in both the U.S. American Life Panel ( Lusardi and Mitchell 2009 ) and the Dutch Central Bank Household Survey (DHS; van Rooij, Lusardi, and Alessie 2011 ). For example, fewer DHS respondents responded correctly when the wording was ‘buying a stock mutual fund usually provides a safer return than a company stock’; conversely, the fraction of correct responses doubled when shown the alternative wording: ‘buying a company stock usually provides a safer return than a stock mutual fund.’ This was not simply due to people using a crude rule of thumb (such as always picking the first as the correct answer), since that would generate a lower rather than a higher percentage of correct answers for version (a). Instead, it appeared that some respondents did not understand the question, perhaps because they were unfamiliar with stocks, bonds, and mutual funds. What this means is that some answers judged to be ‘correct’ may instead be attributable to guessing. In other words, analysis of the financial literacy questions should take into account the possibility that these measures may be noisy proxies of true financial knowledge levels. 23
4. Disaggregating Financial Literacy
To draw out lessons about which people most lack financial knowledge, we turn next to a disaggregated assessment of the data. In what follows, we briefly review evidence by age and sex, race/ethnicity, income and employment status, and other factors of interest to researchers.
Financial Literacy Patterns by Age
The theoretical framework sketched above implies that the life cycle profile of financial literacy will be hump-shaped, and survey data confirm that financial literacy is, in fact, lowest among the young and the old. 24 This is a finding which is robust across countries and we report a selected set of countries in Figure 1 .
Financial Literacy Across Demographic Groups (Age, Sex, and Education)
Note: Data for Figure 1c are taken from Lusardi and Mitchell, 2011d (USA); Alessie, van Rooij, and Lusardi, 2011 (Netherlands), Bucher-Koenen and Lusardi, 2011 (Germany), and Brown and Graf, 2013 (Switzerland).
Of course with cross-sectional data, one cannot cleanly disentangle age from cohort effects, so further analysis is required to identify these clearly, and below we comment further on this point ( Figure 1a ). Nevertheless, it is of interest that older people give themselves very high scores regarding their own financial literacy, despite scoring poorly on the basic financial literacy questions ( Lusardi and Mitchell 2011b ; Lusardi and Tufano 2009a ) and not just in the US but other countries as well ( Lusardi and Mitchell, 2011c ). Similarly, Finke, Howe, and Houston (2011) develop a multidimensional measure of financial literacy for the old and confirm that, though actual financial literacy falls with age, peoples’ confidence in their own financial decision-making abilities actually increases with age. The mismatch between actual and perceived knowledge might explain why financial scams are often perpetrated against the elderly ( Deevy, Lucich, and Beals 2012 ).
Financial Literacy Differences by Sex
One striking feature of the empirical data on financial literacy is the large and persistent gender difference described in Figure 1b . Not only are older men generally more financially knowledgeable than older women, but similar patterns also show up among younger respondents as well ( Lusardi, Mitchell, and Curto 2010 ; Lusardi and Mitchell 2009 ; Lusardi and Tufano 2009a , b ). Moreover, these gaps persist across both the basic and the more sophisticated literacy questions ( Lusardi, Mitchell, and Curto 2012 ; Hung, Parker, and Yoong 2009 ).
One twist on the differences by sex, however, is that while women are less likely to answer financial literacy questions correctly than men, they are also far more likely to say they ‘do not know’ an answer to a question, a result that is strikingly consistent across countries ( Figure 1b ). 25 This awareness of their own lack of knowledge may make women ideal targets for financial education programs.
Because these sex differences in financial literacy are so persistent and widespread across surveys and countries, several researchers have sought to explain them. Consistent with the theoretical framework described earlier, Hsu (2011) proposed that some sex differences may be rational, with specialization of labor within the household leading married women to build up financial knowledge only late in life (close to widowhood). Nonetheless, that study did not explain why financial literacy is also lower among single women in charge of their own finances. Studies of financial literacy in high school and college also revealed sex differences in financial literacy early in life ( Chen and Volpe 2002 ; Mandell 2008 ). 26 Other researchers seeking to explain observed sex differences concluded that traditional explanations cannot fully account for the observed male/female knowledge gap ( Fonseca, Mullen, Zamarro, and Zissimopolous 2012 ; Bucher-Koenen, Lusardi, Alessie, and van Rooij 2012 ). Fonseca, Mullen, Zamarro, and Zissimopoulos (2012) suggested that women may acquire or ‘produce’ financial literacy differently from men, while Bucher-Koenen, Lusardi, Alessie, and van Rooij (2012) pointed to a potentially important role for self-confidence that differs by sex. Brown and Graf (2013) also showed that sex differences are not due to differential interest in finance and financial matters between women and men.
To shed more light on women’s financial literacy, Mahdavi and Horton (2012) examined alumnae from a highly selective U.S. women’s liberal arts college. Even in this talented and well-educated group, women’s financial literacy was found to be very low. In other words, even very well educated women are not particularly financially literate, which could imply that women may acquire financial literacy differently from men. Nevertheless this debate is far from closed, and additional research will be required to better understand these observed sex differences in financial literacy.
Literacy Differences by Education and Ability
As illustrated in Figure 1c , there are substantial differences in financial knowledge by education: specifically, those without a college education are much less likely to be knowledgeable about basic financial literacy concepts, as reported in several U.S. surveys and across countries ( Lusardi and Mitchell 2007a , 2011c ). Moreover, numeracy is especially poor for those with low educational attainment ( Christelis, Jappelli, and Padula 2010 , Lusardi, 2012 ).
How to interpret the finding of a positive link between education and financial savvy has been subject to some debate in the economics literature. One possibility is that the positive correlation might be driven by cognitive ability ( McArdle, Smith, and Willis 2009 ), implying that one must control on measures of ability when seeking to parse out the separate impact of financial literacy. Fortunately, the NLSY has included both measures of financial literacy and of cognitive ability (i.e., the Armed Services Vocational Aptitude Battery). Lusardi, Mitchell, and Curto (2010) did find a positive correlation between financial literacy and cognitive ability among young NLSY respondents, but they also showed that cognitive factors did not fully account for the variance in financial literacy. In other words, substantial heterogeneity in financial literacy remains even after controlling on cognitive factors.
Other Literacy Patterns
There are numerous other empirical regularities in the financial literacy literature, that are again persistent across countries. Financial savvy varies by income and employment type, with lower-paid individuals doing less well and employees and the self-employed doing better than the non-employed ( Lusardi and Tufano 2009a ); Lusardi and Mitchell 2011c ). Several studies have also reported marked differences by race and ethnicity, with African Americans and Hispanics displaying the lowest level of financial knowledge in the U.S. context ( Lusardi and Mitchell 2007a , b , 2011d ). These findings hold across age groups and many different financial literacy measures ( Lusardi and Mitchell 2009 ). Those living in rural areas generally score worse than their city counterparts ( Klapper and Panos 2011 ). These findings might suggest that financial literacy is more easily acquired via interactions with others, in the workplace or in the community. 27 Relatedly, there are also important geographic differences in financial literacy; for example, Fornero and Monticone (2011) report substantial financial literacy dispersion across regions in Italy and so does Beckmann (2013) for Romania Bumcrot, Lin, and Lusardi (2013) report similar differences across U.S. states.
The literature also points to differences in financial literacy by family background. For instance, Lusardi, Mitchell, and Curto (2010) linked financial literacy of 23–28-year-old NLSY respondents to characteristics of the households in which they grew up, controlling for a set of demographic and economic characteristics. Respondents’ financial literacy was also significantly positively correlated with parental education (in particular, that of their mothers), and whether their parents held stocks or retirement accounts when the respondents were teenagers. Mahdavi and Horton (2012) reported a connection between financial literacy and parental background; in this case, fathers’ education was positively associated with their female children’s financial literacy. 28 In other words, financial literacy may well get its start in the family, perhaps when children observe their parents’ saving and investing habits, or more directly by receiving financial education from parents ( Chiteji and Stafford 1999 ; Li 2009 ; Shim, Xiao, Barber, and Lyons 2009 ).
Other studies have noted a nationality gap in financial literacy, with foreign citizens reporting lower financial literacy than the native born ( Brown and Graf 2013 ). Others have found differences in financial literacy according to religion ( Alessie, Van Rooij and Lusardi, 2011 ) and political opinions ( Arrondel, Debbich and Savignac 2013 ). These findings may also shed light on how financial literacy is acquired.
To summarize, while financial illiteracy is widespread, it is also concentrated among specific population sub-groups in most countries studied to date. Such heterogeneity in financial literacy suggests that different mechanisms may be appropriate for tracking the causes and possible consequences of the shortfalls. In the U.S., those facing most challenges are the young and the old, women, African-Americans, Hispanics, the least educated, and those living in rural areas. To date, these differences have not been fully accounted for, though the theoretical framework outlined above provides guidelines for explaining some of these.
5. How Does Financial Literacy Matter?
We turn next to a discussion of whether and how financial literacy matters for economic decision-making. 29 Inasmuch as individuals are increasingly being asked to take on additional responsibility for their own financial well-being, there remains much to learn about these facts. And as we have argued above, when financial literacy itself is a choice variable, it is important to disentangle cause from effect. For instance, those with high net worth who invest in financial markets may also be more likely to care about improving their financial knowledge, since they have more at stake. In what follows, we discuss research linking financial literacy with economic outcomes, taking into account the endogeneity issues as well.
Financial Literacy and Economic Decisions
The early economics literature in this area began by documenting the link between financial literacy and several economic behaviors. For example Bernheim (1995 , 1998 ) was among the first to emphasize that most U.S. households lacked basic financial knowledge and that they also used crude rules of thumb when engaging in saving behavior. More recently, Calvet, Campbell, and Sodini (2007 Calvet, Campbell, and Sodini (2009) evaluated Swedish investors’ actions that they classified as ‘mistakes.’ While that analysis included no direct financial literacy measure, the authors did report that poorer, less educated, and immigrant households (attributes associated with low financial literacy, as noted earlier) were more likely to make financial errors. Agarwal, Driscoll, Gabaix, and Laibson (2009) also focused on financial ‘mistakes’, showing that these were most prevalent among the young and the old, groups which normally display the lowest financial knowledge.
In the wake of the financial crisis of 2008–9, the U.S. federal government has also begun to express substantial concern about another and more extreme case of mistakes, namely where people fall prey to financial scams. As often noted, scams tend to be perpetrated against the elderly, since they are among those with the least financial savvy and often have accumulated some assets. 30 A survey of older financial decision makers (age 60+) indicated that more than half of them reported having made a bad investment, and one in five of those respondents felt they had been misled or defrauded but failed to report the situation ( FINRA 2006 ). As Baby Boomers age, this problem is expected to grow ( Blanton 2012 ), since this cohort is a potentially lucrative target.
Several researchers have examined the relationships between financial literacy and economic behavior. It is much harder to establish a causal link between the two and we will discuss the issue of endogeneity and other problems in more detail below. Hilgert, Hogarth, and Beverly (2003) uncovered a strong correlation between financial literacy and day-to-day financial management skills. Several other studies both in the United States and other countries have found that the more numerate and financially literate are also more likely to participate in financial markets and invest in stocks ( Kimball and Shumway 2006 ; Christelis, Jappelli, and Padula 2010 ; van Rooij, Lusardi, and Alessie 2011 ; Yoong 2011 ; Almenberg and Dreber 2011 ; Arrondel, Debbich, and Savignac 2012 ). Financial literacy can also be linked to holding precautionary savings ( de Bassa Scheresberg 2013 ).
The more financially savvy are also more likely to undertake retirement planning, and those who plan also accumulate more wealth ( Lusardi and Mitchell 2007a , b , 2011a , 2011b ). Some of the first studies on the effects of financial literacy were linked to its effects on retirement planning in the United States and these studies have been replicated in most of the countries covered in Table 2 , showing that the correlation between financial literacy and different measures of retirement planning is quite robust. 31 Studies breaking out specific components of financial literacy tend to conclude that what matters most is advanced financial knowledge (for example, risk diversification) and the capacity to do calculations ( Lusardi and Mitchell 2011d ; Alessie, van Rooij, and Lusardi 2011 ; Fornero and Monticone 2011 ; Klapper and Panos 2011 ; Sekita 2011 ).
Turning to the liability side of the household balance sheet, Moore (2003) reported that the least financially literate are also more likely to have costly mortgages. Campbell (2006) pointed out that those with lower income and less education (characteristics strongly related to financial illiteracy) were less likely to refinance their mortgages during a period of falling interest rates. Stango and Zinman (2009) concluded that those unable to correctly calculate interest rates out of a stream of payments ended up borrowing more and accumulating less wealth. Lusardi and Tufano (2009a ) confirmed that the least financially savvy incurred high transaction costs, paying higher fees and using high-cost borrowing. In their study, the less knowledgeable also reported that their debt loads were excessive, or that they were unable to judge their debt positions. Similarly, Mottola (2013) found that those with low financial literacy were more likely to engage in costly credit card behavior, and Utkus and Young (2011) concluded that the least literate were also more likely to borrow against their 401(k) and pension accounts.
Moreover, both self-assessed and actual literacy is found to have an effect on credit card behavior over the life cycle ( Allgood and Walstad, 2013 ). A particularly well-executed study by Gerardi, Goette, and Meier (2013) matched individual measures of numerical ability to administrative records that provide information on subprime mortgage holders’ payments. Three important findings flowed from this analysis. First, numerical ability was a strong predictor of mortgage defaults. Second, the result persisted even after controlling for cognitive ability and general knowledge. Third, the estimates were quantitatively important, as will be discussed in more detail below, an important finding for both regulators and policymakers.
Many high-cost methods of borrowing have proliferated over time, with negative effects for less savvy consumers. 32 For instance, Lusardi and de Bassa Scheresberg (2013) examined high-cost borrowing in the U.S. including payday loans, pawn shops, auto title loans, refund anticipation loans, and rent-to-own shops. They concluded that the less financially literate were substantially more likely to use high-cost methods of borrowing, a finding that is particularly strong among young adults (age 25–34) ( Bassa Scheresberg 2013 ). While most attention has been devoted to the supply side, these studies suggest it may also be important to look at the demand side and the financial literacy of borrowers. The large number of mortgage defaults during the financial crisis has likewise suggested to some that debt and debt management is a fertile area for mistakes; for instance, many borrowers do not know what interest rates were charged on their credit card or mortgage balances ( Moore 2003 ; Lusardi 2011 ; Disney and Gathergood 2012 ). 33
It is true that education can be quite influential in many of these arenas. For instance, research has shown that the college educated are more likely to own stocks and less prone to use high-cost borrowing ( Haliassos and Bertaut 1995 ; Campbell 2006 ; Lusardi and de Bassa Scheresberg 2012 ). Likewise, there is a very strong positive correlation between education and wealth-holding ( Bernheim and Scholz 1993 ). But for our purposes, including controls for educational attainment in empirical models of stock holding, wealth accumulation, and high-cost methods of borrowing, does not diminish the statistical significance of financial literacy and in fact it often enhances it ( Lusardi and Mitchell 2011b ; Behrman, Mitchell, Soo, and Bravo 2012 ; van Rooij, Lusardi, and Alessie 2011 , 2012 ; Lusardi and de Bassa Scheresberg 2013 ). Evidently, general knowledge (education) and more specialized knowledge (financial literacy) both contribute to more informed financial decision-making. In other words, investment in financial knowledge appears to be a specific form of human capital, rather than being simply associated with more years of schooling. Financial literacy is also linked to the demand for on-the-job training ( Clark, Ogawa, and Matsukura 2010 ) and being able to cope with financial emergencies ( Lusardi, Schneider, and Tufano 2011 ).
Costs of Financial Ignorance Pre-retirement
In the wake of the financial crisis, many have become interested in the costs of financial illiteracy as well as its distributional impacts. For instance, in the Netherlands, van Rooij, Lusardi, and Alessie (2011) estimate that being in the 75 th versus the 25 th percentile of the financial literacy index equals around €80,000 in terms of differential net worth (i.e., roughly 3.5 times the net disposable income of a median Dutch household). They also point out that an increase in financial literacy from the 25 th to the 75 th percentile for an otherwise average individual is associated with a 17–30 percentage point higher probability of stock market participation and retirement planning, respectively. In the U.S., simulations from a life-cycle model that incorporates financial literacy shows that financial literacy alone can explain more than half the observed wealth inequality ( Lusardi, Michaud, and Mitchell 2013 ). This result is obtained by comparing wealth to income ratios across education groups in models with and without financial literacy, which allows individuals to earn higher returns on their savings. For this reason, if the effects of financial literacy on financial behavior can be taken as causal, the costs of financial ignorance are substantial.
In the U.S., investors are estimated to have foregone substantial equity returns due to fees, expenses, and active investment trading costs, in an attempt to ‘beat the market.’ French (2008) calculates that this amounts to an annual total cost of around $100 billion, which could be avoided by passive indexing. Since the least financially literate are unlikely to be sensitive to fees, they are most likely to bear such costs. Additionally, many of the financially illiterate have been shown to shun the stock market, which Cocco, Gomes, and Maenhout (2005) suggested imposed welfare losses amounting to four percent of wealth. The economic cost of under-diversification computed by Calvet, Campbell, and Sodini (2007) is also substantial: they concluded that a median investor in Sweden experienced an annual return loss of 2.9 percent on a risky portfolio, or 0.5 percent of household disposable income. But for one in 10 investors, these annual costs were much higher, 4.5 percent of disposable income.
Costs of financial ignorance arise not only in the saving and investment arena, but also influence how consumers manage their liabilities. Campbell (2006) reported that suboptimal refinancing among U.S. homeowners resulted in 0.5–1 percent per year higher mortgage interest rates, or in aggregate, $50–100 billion annually. And as noted above, the least financially savvy are least likely to refinance their mortgages. Gerardi, Goette, and Meier (2013) showed that numerical ability may have contributed substantially to the massive defaults on subprime mortgages in the recent financial crisis. According to their estimates, those in the highest numerical ability grouping had about a 20 percentage point lower probability of defaulting on their subprime mortgages than those in the lowest financial numeracy group.
One can also link ‘debt literacy’ regarding credit card behaviors that generate fees and interest charges to paying bills late, going over the credit limit, using cash advances, and paying only the minimum amount due. Lusardi and Tufano (2009a ) calculated the “cost of ignorance” or transaction costs incurred by less-informed Americans and the component of these costs related to lack of financial knowledge. Their calculation of expected costs had two components—the likelihood and the costs of various credit card behaviors. These likelihoods were derived directly from empirical estimates using the data on credit card behavior, debt literacy, and a host of demographic controls that include income. They showed that, while less knowledgeable individuals constitute only 29 percent of the cardholder population, they accounted for 42 percent of these charges. Accordingly, the least financially savvy bear a disproportionate share of the costs associated with fee-inducing behaviors. Indeed, the average fees paid by those with low knowledge were 50 percent higher than those paid by the average cardholder. And of these four types of charges incurred by less-knowledgeable cardholders, one-third were incremental charges linked to low financial literacy.
Another way that the financially illiterate spend dearly for financial services is via high-cost forms of borrowing, including payday loans. 34 While the amount borrowed is often low ($300 on average), such loans are often made to individuals who have five or more such transactions per year ( Center for Responsible Lending 2004 ). It turns out that these borrowers also frequently fail to take advantage of other, cheaper opportunities to borrow. Agarwal, Skiba, and Tobacman (2009) studied payday borrowers who also have access to credit cards, and they found that two-thirds of their sample had at least $1,000 in credit card liquidity on the day they took out their first payday loan. This points to a pecuniary mistake: given average charges for payday loans and credit cards and considering a two-week payday loan of $300, the use of credit cards would have saved these borrowers substantial amounts – around $200 per year (and more if they took out repeated payday loans). While there may be good economic reasons why some people may want to keep below their credit card limits, including unexpected shocks, Bertrand and Morse (2011) determined that payday borrowers often labored under cognitive biases, similar to those with low financial literacy ( Lusardi and de Bassa Scheresberg 2013 ).
Costs of Financial Ignorance in Retirement
Financial knowledge impacts key outcomes including borrowing, saving, and investing decisions not only during the worklife, but afterwards, in retirement, as well. In view of the fact that people over the age of 65 hold more than $18 trillion in wealth, 35 this is an important issue.
Above we noted that financial literacy is associated with greater retirement planning and greater retirement wealth accumulation. 36 Hence it stands to reason that the more financially savvy will likely be better financially endowed when they do retire. A related point is that the more financially knowledgeable are also better informed about pension system rules, pay lower investment fees in their retirement accounts, and diversify their pension assets better ( Arenas de Mesa, Bravo, Behrman, Mitchell, and Todd 2008 ; Chan and Stevens 2008 ; Hastings, Mitchell, and Chyn 2011 ). 37 To date, however, relatively little has been learned about whether more financially knowledgeable older adults are also more successful at managing their resources in retirement, though the presence of scams among the elderly suggests that this topic is highly policy-relevant.
This is a particularly difficult set of decisions requiring retirees to look ahead to an uncertain future when making irrevocable choices with far-reaching consequences. For instance, people must forecast their (and their partner’s) survival probabilities, investment returns, pension income, and medical and other expenditures. Moreover, many of these financial decisions are once-in-a-lifetime events, including when to retire and claim one’s pension and Social Security benefits. Accordingly, it would not be surprising if financial literacy enhanced peoples’ ability to make these important and consequential decisions.
This question is especially relevant when it comes to the decision of whether retirees purchase lifetime income streams with their assets, since by so doing, they insure themselves from running out of income in old age. 38 Nevertheless, despite the fact that this form of longevity protection is very valuable in theory, relatively few payout annuities are purchased in practice in virtually every country ( Mitchell, Piggott, and Takayama 2011 ). New research points to the importance of framing and default effects in this decision process ( Agnew and Szkyman 2011 ; Brown, Kapteyn, and Mitchell 2013 ). This conclusion was corroborated by Brown, Kapteyn, Luttmer, and Mitchell (2011) , who demonstrated experimentally that people valued annuities less when they were offered the opportunity to buy additional income streams, and they valued annuities more if offered a chance to exchange their annuity flows for a lump sum. 39 Importantly for the present purpose, the financially savvy provided more consistent responses across alternative ways of eliciting preferences. By contrast, the least financially literate gave inconsistent results and respond to irrelevant cues when presented with the same set of choices. In other words, financial literacy appears to be highly influential in helping older households equip themselves with longevity risk protection in retirement.
Much more must be learned about how peoples’ financial decision-making abilities change with age, and how these are related to financial literacy. For instance, Agarwal, Driscoll, Gabaix, and Laibson (2009) reported that the elderly pay much more than the middle-aged for 10 financial products; 40 the 75-year-olds in their sample paid about $265 more per year for home equity lines of credit than did the 50-year-olds. How the patterns might vary by financial literacy is not yet known, but it might be that those with greater baseline financial knowledge are better able to deal with financial decisions as they move into the second half of their lifetimes. 41
Coping with Endogeneity and Measurement Error
Despite an important assembly of facts on financial literacy, relatively few empirical analysts have accounted for the potential endogeneity of financial literacy and the problem of measurement error in financial literacy alluded to above. In the last five years or so, however, several authors have implemented instrumental variables (IV) estimation to assess the impact of financial literacy on financial behavior, and the results tend to be quite convincing. To illustrate the ingenuity of the instruments used, Table 4 lists several studies along with the instruments used in their empirical analysis. Some of the descriptive evidence on financial literacy discussed earlier may explain why these instruments may be anticipated to predict financial literacy.
Instrumental Variable (IV) Estimation of the Effect of Financial Literacy on Behavior
It is useful to offer a handful of comments on some of the papers with particularly strong instruments. Christiansen, Joensen, and Rangvid (2008) used the opening of a new university in a local area—arguably one of the most exogenous variables one can find— as instrument for knowledge, and they concluded that economics education is an important determinant of investment in stocks. Following this lead, Klapper, Lusardi and Panos (2012) used the number of public and private universities in the Russian regions and the total number of newspapers in circulation as instruments for financial literacy. They found that financial literacy affected a variety of economic indicators including having bank accounts, using bank credit, using informal credit, having spending capacity, and the availability of unspent income. Lusardi and Mitchell (2009) instrumented financial literacy using the fact that different U.S. states mandated financial education in high school in different states and at different points in time and they interacted these mandates with state expenditures on education. Behrman, Mitchell, Soo, and Bravo (2012) employed several instruments including exposure to a new educational voucher system in Chile to isolate the causal effects of financial literacy and schooling attainment on wealth. Their IV results showed that both financial literacy and schooling attainment were positively and significantly associated with wealth levels.
Van Rooij, Lusardi, and Alessie (2011) instrumented financial literacy with the financial experiences of siblings and parents, since these were arguably not under respondents’ control, to rigorously evaluate the relationship between financial literacy and stock market participation. The authors reported that instrumenting greatly enhanced the measured positive impact of financial literacy on stock market participation. These instruments were also recently used by Agnew, Bateman and Thorp (2013) to assess the effect of financial literacy on retirement planning in Australia. Bucher-Koenen and Lusardi (2011) used political attitudes at the regional level in Germany as an instrument, arguing that free-market oriented supporters are more likely to be financially literate, and the assumption is that individuals can learn from others around them. The study by Arrondel, Debbich, and Savignac (2013) also shows some differences in financial literacy across political affiliation.
Interestingly, in all these cases, the IV financial literacy estimates always proves to be larger than the ordinary least squares estimates ( Table 4 ). This might be that people affected by the instruments have large responses or there is severe measurement error, but on the other hand, it seems clear that the non-instrumented estimates of financial literacy may underestimate the true effect.
Despite these advances, one might worry that other omitted variables could still influence financial decisions in ways that could bias results. For example, unobservables such as discount rates ( Meier and Sprenger 2008 ), IQ ( Grinblatt, Keloharju, and Linnainmaa 2011 ), or cognitive abilities could influence saving decisions and portfolio choice ( Delavande, Rohwedder, and Willis 2008 ; Korniotis and Kumar 2011 ). If these cannot be controlled for, estimated financial literacy impacts could be biased. However, Alessie, van Rooij, and Lusardi’s (2011) work using panel data and fixed-effects regression as well as IV estimation confirmed the positive effect of financial literacy on retirement planning, and several studies, as mentioned earlier (c.f., Gerardi, Goette and Meier 2013 ), account explicitly for cognitive ability. Nevertheless, they show that numeracy has an effect above and beyond cognitive ability.
A different way to parse out the effects of financial literacy on economic outcomes is to use a field experiment in which one group of individuals (the treatment group) is exposed to a financial education program and their behavior is then compared to that of a second group not thus exposed (the control group). Yet even in countries with less developed financial markets and pension systems, financial literacy impacts are similar to those found when examining the effect of financial literacy on retirement planning and pension participation ( Lusardi and Mitchell 2011c ). For example, Song (2011) showed that learning about interest compounding produces a sizeable increase in pension contributions in China. Randomized experimental studies in Mexico and Chile demonstrated that more financially literate individuals were more likely to choose pension accounts with lower administrative fees ( Hastings and Tejeda-Ashton 2008 ; Hastings and Mitchell 2011 ; Hastings, Mitchell, and Chyn 2011 ). More financially sophisticated individuals in Brazil were also less affected by their peers’ choices in their financial decisions ( Bursztyn, Ederer, Ferman, and Yuchtman 2013 ).
The financial crisis has also provided a laboratory to study the effects of financial literacy against a backdrop of economic shocks. For example, when stock markets dropped sharply around the world, investors were exposed to large losses in their portfolios. This combined with much higher unemployment has made it even more important to be savvy in managing limited resources. Bucher-Koenen and Ziegelmeyer (2011) examined the financial losses experienced by German households during the financial crisis and confirmed that the least financially literate were more likely to sell assets that had lost value, thus locking in losses. 42 In Russia, Klapper, Lusardi, and Panos (2012) found that the most financially literate were significantly less likely to report having experienced diminished spending capacity and had more available saving. Additionally, estimates from different time periods implied that financial literacy better equips individuals to deal with macroeconomic shocks.
Given this evidence on the negative outcomes and costs of financial illiteracy, we turn next to financial education programs to remedy these shortfalls.
6. Assessing the Effects of Financial Literacy Programs
Another way to assess the effects of financial literacy is to look at the evidence on financial education programs whose aims and objectives are to improve financial knowledge. Financial education programs in the U.S. and elsewhere have been implemented over the years in several different settings: in schools, workplaces, and libraries, and sometimes population subgroups have been targeted. As one example, several U.S. states mandated financial education in high school at different points in time, generating ‘natural experiments’ utilized by Bernheim, Garrett, and Maki (2001) , one of the earliest studies in this literature. Similarly, financial education in high schools has recently been examined in Brazil and Italy ( Bruhn, Legovini, and Zia 2012 ; Romagnoli and Trifilidis 2012 ). In some instances, large U.S. firms have launched financial education programs (c.f. Bernheim and Garrett (2003) , Clark and D’Ambrosio (2008) , and Clark, Morrill, and Allen (2012a , b )). Often the employer’s intention is to boost defined benefit pension plan saving and participation ( Duflo and Saez 2003 , 2004 ; Lusardi, Keller, and Keller 2008 ; Goda, Manchester, and Sojourner 2012 ). Programs have also been adopted for especially vulnerable groups such as those in financial distress ( Collins and O’Rourke 2010 ).
Despite the popularity of the programs, only a few authors have undertaken careful evaluations of the impact of financial education programs. Rather than detailing or reviewing the existing literature, 43 here we instead draw attention to the key issues which future researchers must take into account when evaluating the effectiveness of financial education programs. 44 We also highlight key recent research not reviewed in prior surveys.
A concern emphasized above in Section 2 is that evaluation studies have sometimes been conducted without a clear understanding of how financial knowledge is developed. That is, if we define financial literacy as a form of human capital investment, it stands to reason that some will find it optimal to invest in financial literacy while others will not. Accordingly, if a program were to be judged based on specific behavioral changes such as increasing retirement saving or participation in retirement accounts, it should be recognized that the program is unlikely, both theoretically and practically, to change everyone’s behavior in the same way. 45 For example, a desired outcome from a financial education program might be to boost saving. Yet for some, it may not be optimal to save; for others, it might be rational to reduce debt. Hence, unless an evaluator focused on the household portfolio problem including broader saving measures, a program might (incorrectly) be judged a failure.
A related concern is that, since such a large portion of the population is not financially knowledgeable about even the basic concepts of interest compounding, inflation, and risk diversification, it is unlikely that short exposure to financial literacy training would make much of a dent in consumers’ decision-making prowess. For this reason, offering a few retirement seminars or sending employees to a benefit fair can be fairly ineffective ( Duflo and Saez 2003 , 2004 ). Additionally, few studies have undertaken a careful cost-benefit analysis, which should be a high priority for future research.
The evidence reported previously also shows there is substantial heterogeneity in both financial literacy and financial behavior, so that programs targeting specific groups are likely to be more effective than one-size-fits-all financial education programs. For example, Lusardi, Michaud and Mitchell (2013) show theoretically that there is substantial heterogeneity in individual behavior, implying that not everyone will gain from financial education. Accordingly, saving will optimally be zero (or negative) for some, and financial education programs in this case would not be expected to change that behavior. In other words, one should not expect a 100 percent participation rate in financial education programs. In this respect, the model delivers an important prediction: in order to change behavior, financial education programs must be targeted to specific groups of the population since people have different preferences and economic circumstances.
As in other fields of economic research, program evaluations must also be rigorous if they are to persuasively establish causality and effectiveness. As noted by Collins and O’Rourke (2010) , the ‘golden rule’ of evaluation is the experimental approach in which a ‘treatment’ group exposed to financial literacy education is compared with a ‘control’ group that is not (or that is exposed to a different treatment). Thus far, as noted above, few financial educational programs have been designed or evaluated with these standards in mind, making it difficult to draw inferences. A related point is that confounding factors may bias estimated impacts unless the evaluation is carefully structured. As an example, we point to the debate over the efficacy of teaching financial literacy in high school, a discussion that will surely be fed by the new financial literacy module in the 2012 PISA mentioned above. Some have argued against financial education in school (e.g., Willis 2008 ), drawing on the findings from the Jump$tart Coalition for Personal Financial Literacy ( Mandell 2004 , 2008 ). The Jump$tart studies concluded that students scored no better in financial literacy tests even if they attended school in states having financial education; in fact, in some cases, Mandell (1997 , 2008 ) found that they scored even worse than students in states lacking these programs. Yet subsequent analyses ( Walstad, Rebeck, and MacDonald 2010 ) pointed out that this research was incomplete as it did not account for course content, test measurement, teacher preparation, and amount of instruction. These points were underscored by Tennyson and Nguyen (2001) who revisited the Jump$tart data by looking more closely at state education requirements for personal finance education. They concluded that when students were mandated to take a financial education course, they perform much better than students in states with no personal finance mandates. Accordingly, there is reason to believe that mandating personal finance education may, in fact, be effective in increasing student knowledge—but only when it requires significant exposure to personal finance concepts.
It is likewise risky to draw inferences without knowing about the quality of teaching in these courses. For instance, Way and Holden (2009) examined over 1,200 K–12 teachers, prospective teachers, and teacher education faculty representing four U.S. census regions, along with teachers’ responses to questions about their personal and educational backgrounds in financial education. Almost all of the teachers recognized the importance of and need for financial education, yet fewer than one-fifth stated they were prepared to teach any of the six personal finance concepts normally included in the educational rubrics. Furthermore, prospective teachers felt least competent in the more technical topics including risk management and insurance, as well as saving and investing. Interestingly, these are also the concepts that the larger adult population struggles with, as noted above. That study concluded that state education mandates appeared to have no effect on whether teachers took courses in personal finance, taught the courses, or felt competent to teach such a course, consistent with the fact that the states mandating high school financial education did not necessarily provide or promote teacher training in the field.
It would also be valuable to further investigate whether the knowledge scores actually measured what was taught in school and whether students self-selected into the financial education classes. Walstad, Rebeck, and MacDonald (2010) used a quasi-experimental set up to assess a well-designed video course covering several fundamental concepts for both students and teachers. The test they employed was aligned with what was taught in school, and it measured students’ initial levels of understanding of personal finance so as to capture improvements in financial knowledge. Results indicated a significant increase in personal finance knowledge among the ‘treated’ students, suggesting that carefully crafted experiments can and do detect important improvements in knowledge. This is an area that would benefit from additional careful evaluative research ( Collins and O’Rourke 2010 ).
Compared to the research on schooling, evaluating workplace financial education seems even more challenging. There is evidence that employees who attended a retirement seminar were much more likely to save and contribute to their pension accounts ( Bernheim and Garrett 2003 ). Yet those who attended such seminars could be a self-selected group, since attendance was voluntary; that is, they might already have had a proclivity to save.
Another concern is that researchers have often little or no information on the content and quality of the workplace seminars. A few authors have measured the information content of the seminars ( Clark and D’Ambrosio 2008 ; Lusardi, Keller, and Keller 2008 ) and conducted pre- and post- evaluations to detect behavioral changes or intentions to change future behavior. Their findings, including in-depth interviews and qualitative analysis, are invaluable for shedding light on how to make programs more effective. One notable recent experiment involved exposing a representative sample of the U.S. population to short videos explaining several fundamental concepts including the power of interest compounding, inflation, risk diversification, all topics that most people fail to comprehend ( Heinberg, Hung, Kapteyn, Lusardi, and Yoong 2010 ). Compared to a control group who did not receive such education, those exposed to the informational videos were more knowledgeable and better able to answer hypothetical questions about saving decisions. 46 While more such research is needed, when researchers target concepts using carefully-designed experiments, they are more likely to detect changes in knowledge and behavior critical for making financial decisions.
A related challenge is that it may be difficult to evaluate empirically how actual workers’ behavior changes after an experimental treatment of the type just discussed. Goda, Manchester, and Sojourner (2012) asked whether employee decisions to participate in and contribute to their company retirement plan were affected by information about the correlation between retirement savings and post-retirement income. Since the computation involves complex relationships between contributions, investment returns, retirement ages, and longevity, this is an inherently difficult decision. In that study, employees were randomly assigned to control and treatment groups; the treatment group received an information intervention while nothing was sent to the control group. The intervention contained projections of the additional account balance and retirement income that would result from additional hypothetical contribution amounts, customized to each employee’s current age. Results showed that the treatment group members were more likely than the control group to boost their pension contributions and contribution rates; the increase was an additional 0.17 percent of salary. Moreover, the treatment group felt better informed about retirement planning and was more likely to have figured out how much to save. This experiment is notable in that it rigorously illustrates the effectiveness of interventions—even low-cost informational ones—in increasing pension participation and contributions. 47
Very promising work assessing the effects of financial literacy has also begun to emerge from developing countries. Frequently analysts have focused on people with very low financial literacy and in vulnerable subgroups which may have the most to gain. Many of these studies have also used the experimental method described above, now standard in development economics research. These studies contribute to an understanding of the mechanisms driving financial literacy as well as economic advances for financial education program participants. One example, by Carpena, Cole, Shapiro, and Zia (2011) , sought to disentangle how financial literacy programs influence financial behavior. The authors used a randomized experiment on low income urban households in India who underwent a five-week comprehensive video-based financial education program with modules on savings, credit, insurance and budgeting. They concluded that financial education in this context did not increase respondent numeracy, perhaps not surprisingly given that only four percent of respondents had a secondary education. Nevertheless, financial education did positively influence participant awareness of and attitudes toward financial products and financial planning tools.
In a related study, Cole, Giné, Tobacman, Topalova, Townsend, and Vickery (2013) found that demand for rainfall insurance was higher in villages where individuals were more financially literate. Cai, de Janvry, and Sadoulet (2013) showed that lack of financial education was a major constraint on the demand for weather insurance in rural China and that financial training could significantly improve take-up rates. Moreover, Song (2011) showed that when Chinese farmers were taught about interest compounding, it produced a sizeable increase in pension contributions. 48 This is encouraging given the evidence, even in developing countries, of lack of knowledge about interest compounding and the preliminary results on teaching this concept using videos.
In sum, while much effort has been devoted to examining the effectiveness of financial education programs in a variety of settings, relatively few studies have been informed by either a suitable theoretical model or a carefully-designed empirical approach. And since the theory predicts that not everyone will invest in financial knowledge, it is unreasonable to expect all ‘treated’ by a program will dramatically change their behavior. Moreover, a short program that is not tailored to specific groups’ needs is unlikely to make much difference. For these reasons, future analysts would do well to emulate the more recent rigorous field experiments that trace how both knowledge and behavior changes result from additional purpose-designed financial information and training.
7. Implications and Discussion
As we have shown, a relatively parsimonious set of questions measuring basic concepts such as interest compounding, inflation, and risk diversification has now become the starting point for evaluating levels of financial literacy around the world. Using these questions, researchers have demonstrated that low levels of financial knowledge are pervasive, suggesting that it will be quite challenging to provide the tools to help people function more effectively in complex financial and credit markets requiring sophisticated financial decision-making. While research in this field continues to spread, it seems clear that there are likely to be important benefits of greater financial knowledge, including savvier saving and investment decisions, better debt management, more retirement planning, higher participation in the stock market, and greater wealth accumulation. Though it is challenging to establish a causal link between financial literacy and economic behavior, both instrumental variables and experimental approaches suggest that financial literacy plays a role in influencing financial decision making, and the causality goes from knowledge to behavior.
Much work remains to be done. Very importantly, there has been no carefully-crafted cost-benefit analysis indicating which sorts of financial education programs are most appropriate, and least expensive, for which kinds of people. Some research from developing countries speaks to this point, comparing educational treatments with other approaches such as simplifying decisions ( Cole, Sampson, and Zia 2011 ; Drexel, Fischer, and Schoar 2011 ), but this remains a high priority area. In any event, the estimated aggregate costs of financial illiteracy point to possibly high returns, especially in the areas of consumer debt and debt management.
A related issue has to do with which sorts of problems are best suited to remedying through financial education, versus removing choice options from consumers’ menus altogether or simplifying the options that people face. In this vein, Thaler and Sunstein (2010) have emphasized the importance of devoting careful attention to the design of the environments in which people make choices, or the so-called ‘choice architecture.’ An important example arises in the context of employer-provided pensions, which in the past left it to individual employees to decide whether to save and how to invest their defined contribution contributions. When employers automatically enroll workers into these plans rather than let them opt in, this can dramatically increase pension participation (from less than 40 to close to 90 percent, as reported in one of the seminal work in this area, i.e., Madrian and Shea 2001 ). Several other studies also note that automatic enrollment leads to large and persistent increases in pension participation ( Choi, Laibson, and Madrian 2004 ; Choi, Laibson, Madrian and Metrick 2006 ; Thaler and Benartzi 2004 ), and better diversified portfolios ( Mitchell and Utkus 2012 ).
Moreover, in the wake of the recent financial crisis, attention has been increasingly devoted to methods of protecting people from their own financial illiteracy and inability to make informed financial decisions. The fact that unsophisticated consumers may not appreciate and take advantage of the many opportunities offered by complex financial markets leaves them at the mercy of scams ( Deevy, Lucich, and Beals 2012 ) and in turn, has given rise to protective legislation. For instance the Dodd-Frank Act of 2010, establishing the U.S. Consumer Financial Protection Bureau, had as a key goal the development of a government entity that could better protect consumers and specify uniform standards for financial products. 49 Campbell, Jackson, Madrian, and Tufano (2011) recently reviewed the theoretical and empirical consumer protection literature, making a case for consumer financial regulation. As they noted, in a system of individual responsibility where individuals must make important economic decisions instead of having governments and employers do so centrally, it will be important to reduce search costs, for example via standardized and centralized information. Similarly, for contracts or decisions that people engage in infrequently (such as buying a home or saving for retirement) and where there are few chances to learn from experience, it may be useful to structure the information provided and make it easily understood.
The debate about the role of regulation versus financial education is still ongoing. In our view, it would be useful to enhance cross-fertilization between behavioral economics and its focus on choice architecture, and the group proposing to educate people about financial basics; that is, it need not be an ‘either/or’ choice. Similar, regulation and financial education are not necessarily substitutes, as they can also complement each other. 50 As Thaler, Sunstein, and Balz (2010 : np) note: ‘choice architects do not always have the best interests of the people they are influencing in mind.’ Moreover, expanding automatic enrollment to the decumulation phase by implementing automatic annuitization of pensions upon retirement (a topic of current policy debate) might be deleterious to those having to cut consumption during their work lives and render some ineligible for government benefit programs after retirement (such as Medicaid or Supplemental Security Income). Likewise, pension plan sponsors have tended to establish very low saving targets in their default auto-enrollment arrangements, fearing that employees might not participate in their plans if the default contribution rates were high. For instance, auto-enrollment contribution rates for new hires in the paper by Madrian and Shea (2001) mentioned earlier, were set at three percent of salary, whereas a six percent contribution rate would have entitled workers to receive a 50 percent employer match. In that setting, the low default saving rate did not prod workers to take full advantage of the employer match. 51 Moreover, the three percent default set by the firm was taken by employees as a signal of a ‘suggested target’ saving level, since many of them reduced their contributions to three percent even if they had saved more previously. Additional examples of people treating the default as an employer-endorsed target include Beshears, Choi, Laibson, and Madrian (2012) , who showed that workers tended to stick to the ‘wrong’ default for long periods of time. Interestingly, those likely to do so were disproportionately low income and less educated, those likely to be the least financially literate.
The human capital approach to financial literacy suggests that there will be substantial heterogeneity in both financial knowledge and economic behavior, so it is unlikely that any one default rate or environment will enhance wellbeing for everyone. Thus if workers are carrying credit card debt or high-interest mortgages, it may be more sensible to pay off these debts rather than raise their pension contributions. Similarly, borrowing from one’s 401(k) plan may be more cost-effective for financially strapped households, versus taking out higher-cost debt elsewhere ( Lu, Mitchell, and Utkus 2010 ). And of course, only about half of the U.S. workforce is employed at firms that offer pensions, so the remaining several million employees without pensions would not benefit from automatic enrollment.
If, as argued previously, saving decisions are very complex, one way to help people save may be to find ways to simplify those decisions. For example, it could be useful to find ways to move people to action. Such a strategy is analyzed by Choi, Laibson, and Madrian (2004) , who studied the effects of Quick Enrollment, a program that gave workers the option of enrolling in the employer-provided saving plan by opting into a preset default contribution rate and asset allocation. Here, and unlike the default scenario, workers had a choice of whether or not to enroll, but the decision was much simplified as they did not need to set their contribution rates or how to allocate their assets.
Another approach designed to simplify the decision to save and, in addition, motivate employees to make an active choice involves a planning aid distributed to new hires during employee orientation ( Lusardi, Keller, and Keller 2008 ). This planning aid broke down the process of enrolling in supplementary pensions into several small steps, describing to participants what they needed to do to be able to enroll online. It also provided several pieces of information to help overcome barriers to saving, such as describing the low minimum amount of income employees can contribute (in addition to the maximum) and indicating the default fund that the employer has chosen for them (a life-cycle fund). While the program evaluation was not performed in an experimental setting, the study provided several useful insights. The qualitative data collected reveals important heterogeneity across employees, even within the same firm. Results also showed that economic incentives such as employer matches or tax advantages need not exhaust the list of options to induce people to save. The authors also concluded that employees were more prone to decision-making at some times rather than others. For example, starting a new job is a good time to think about saving, often because people must make decisions about their pension contributions.
In the developing country context, more work is also needed to assess whether simplification can help uneducated individuals make better financial decisions. This can include using simple financial instruments such as checking accounts, to more complex contracts such as insurance and decisions related to entrepreneurial activities. Early research has been promising: Drexel, Fischer, and Schoar (2011) showed that a simplified rule-of-thumb training program enhanced business practices and outcomes among microentrepreneurs in the Dominican Republic. Kast, Meier, and Pomeranz (2012) also found that self-help peer groups and text messaging boosted employee saving patterns in Chile.
An alternative method of enhancing peoples’ performance in an increasingly financially complex world might be to outsource the job, by relying on financial advice. Some have argued it is not feasible or even desirable to make everyone be a financial expert ( Willis 2008 , 2011 ). Of course financial education programs do not turn ordinary consumers into experts, just as courses on literature do not make students into professional writers. Also individuals must make many financial decisions not requiring professional advice from opening checking accounts to paying credit cards. Yet some decisions, such as saving for retirement and making investment choices, do require rather sophisticated knowledge, so turning to advisors could be desirable. In the U.S., at least, only a small fraction of households currently consults financial advisors, bankers, certified public accountants, or other such advice professionals, with most still relying on informal sources of advice ( Mitchell and Smetters 2013 ). Even among those who indicate they might be willing to use professional investment advice, two-thirds state they would probably implement only those recommendations that were in line with their own ideas ( Employee Benefit Research Institute 2007 ). In other words, financial advice might not have a large impact if individuals fail to seek out and act on the recommendations of their advisors.
Additionally, there are many different types of ‘advice professional’ credentials, each regulated by different private and/or public sector entities. Accordingly it may be difficult or even impossible for consumers to determine whether the quality of advice provided is accurate, suitable, and consistent with their own goals. For instance, advisor compensation structures sometimes are not well-aligned with household interests. And those least likely to be knowledgeable may also face obstacles in identifying good advice sources: for example, Collins (2011) and Finke (2013) argued that financial literacy and financial advice are complements rather than substitutes. 52
Relatively little is known about the effects of financial advice and whether it can improve financial decision-making. Some preliminary evidence suggests that financial counseling can be effective in reducing debt levels and delinquency rates ( Agarwal, Amromin, Ben-David, Chomsisengphet, and Evanoff 2011 ; Collins and O’Rouke 2010 ; Elliehausen, Lundquist, and Staten 2007 ; and Hirad and Zorn 2002 ). In practice, however, most people continue to rely on the help of family and friends for their financial decisions.
8. Conclusions and Remaining Questions
In the wake of the global financial crisis, policymakers around the world have expressed deep concern about widespread lack of financial knowledge. Efforts are also underway to fill these gaps with specific programs to ‘identify individuals who are most in need of financial education and the best ways to improve that education’ ( OECD 2005 ). The U.S. President’s Advisory Council on Financial Literacy ( PACFL 2008 , np) noted that ‘far too many Americans do not have the basic financial skills necessary to develop and maintain a budget, to understand credit, to understand investment vehicles, or to take advantage of our banking system. It is essential to provide basic financial education that allows people to better navigate an economic crisis such as this one.’ U.S. Federal Reserve Board Chairman Bernanke (2011 : 2) has similarly opined: ‘In our dynamic and complex financial marketplace, financial education must be a lifelong pursuit that enables consumers of all ages and economic positions to stay attuned to changes in their financial needs and circumstances and to take advantage of products and services that best meet their goals. Well-informed consumers, who can serve as their own advocates, are one of the best lines of defense against the proliferation of financial products and services that are unsuitable, unnecessarily costly, or abusive.’
Despite policy agreement on the need to fill these gaps, analysts and policymakers have much to learn about the most cost-effective ways to build financial knowledge in the population at large. The literature to date has showed that many people are financially illiterate, around the world, as we have sketched here. Econometric models and experiments have done much to confirm the causal impact of financial literacy on economic decision-making, and to separately identify this effect from other factors, including education and cognitive ability. Research on efforts to enhance financial literacy suggest that some interventions work well, but additional experimental work is critical to control for endogeneity and confirm causality.
Several key tasks remain. First, theoretical models of saving and financial decision-making must be further enriched to incorporate the fact that financial knowledge is a form of human capital. Second, efforts to better measure financial education are likely to pay off, including gathering information on teachers, training programs, and material covered. Third, outcomes beyond what have been studied to date are likely to be of interest, including borrowing for student loans, investment in health, reverse mortgage patterns, and when to claim Social Security benefits, decisions that all have far-reaching economic consequences. Additional experimental research would be useful, to learn more about the directions of causality between financial knowledge and economic wellbeing, though the early results offered here are promising. While the costs of raising financial literacy are likely to be substantial, so too are the costs of being liquidity-constrained, over-indebted, and poor.
Acknowledgments
The research reported herein was performed pursuant to a grant from the TIAA-CREF Institute; additional research support was provided by the Pension Research Council and Boettner Center at the Wharton School of the University of Pennsylvania. The authors thank the editor, Janet Currie, four anonymous referees, and Tabea Bucher-Koenen, Pierre-Carl Michaud, Maarten van Rooij, and Stephen Utkus for suggestions and comments, and Carlo de Bassa Scheresberg, Hugh Kim, Donna St. Louis, and Yong Yu for research assistance. Opinions and conclusions expressed herein are solely those of the authors and do not represent the opinions or policy of the funders or any other institutions with which the authors are affiliated.
See Lusardi (2011) and FINRA Investor Education Foundation (2009 and FINRA Investor Education Foundation (2012).
In the early 1980’s, around 40 percent of U.S. private-sector pension contributions went to DC plans; two decades later, almost 90 percent of such contributions went to retirement accounts (mostly 401(k) plans; Poterba, Venti, and Wise 2008 ).
See, for instance, Brown, Kapteyn, and Mitchell (2013)
For an older review of the saving literature see Browning and Lusardi (1996) ; recent surveys are provided by Skinner (2007) and Attanasio and Weber (2010) . A very partial list of the literature discussing new theoretical advances includes Cagetti (2003) ; Chai, Horneff, Maurer, and Mitchell (2012) ; DeNardi, French, and Jones (2011) ; French (2005 , 2008 ); Gourinchas and Parker (2002) ; Hurst and Aguiar (2005 Hurst and Aguiar (2007) ; and Scholz, Seshadri, and Khitatrakun (2006) .
Glewwe (2002) and Hanusheck and Woessman (2008) review the economic impacts of schooling and cognitive development.
Another related study is by Benitez-Silva, Demiralp, and Liu (2009) who use a dynamic life cycle model of optimal Social Security benefit claiming against which they compare outcomes to those generated under a suboptimal information structure where people simply copy those around them when deciding when to claim benefits. The authors do not, however, allow for endogenous acquisition of information.
This cost function is assumed to be convex, though the authors also experiment with alternative formulations, which do not materially alter results. Kézdi and Willis (2011) also model heterogeneity in beliefs about the stock market, where people can learn about the statistical process governing stock market returns, reducing transactions costs for investments. Here, however, the investment cost was cast as a simplified flat fixed fee per person, whereas Lusardi, Michaud, and Mitchell (2013) evaluate more complex functions of time and money costs for investments in knowledge.
There is also a minimum consumption floor; see Lusardi, Michaud, and Mitchell (2011 , 2013 ).
Assets must be non-negative each period and there is a nonzero mortality probability as well as a finite length of life.
Additional detail on calibration and solution methods can be found in Lusardi, Mitchell, and Michaud (2011 in Lusardi, Mitchell, and Michaud (2013) .
Initial conditions for education, earnings, and assets are derived from Panel Study of Income Dynamics (PSID) respondents age 25–30.
This approach could account for otherwise “unexplained” wealth inequality discussed by Venti and Wise (1998 Venti and Wise (2001) .
These predictions directly contradict at least one lawyer’s surmise that “[i]n an idealized first-best world, where all people are far above average, education would train every consumer to be financially literate and would motivate every consumer to use that literacy to make good choices” ( Willis 2008 ).
See Huston (2010) for a review of financial literacy measures.
For information about the HRS, see http://hrsonline.isr.umich.edu/
Information on the 2009 and 2012 National Financial Capability Study can be found here: http://www.usfinancialcapability.org/
Other financial knowledge measures include Kimball and Shumway (2006) , Lusardi and Mitchell (2009) , Yoong (2011) , Hung, Parker, and Yoong (2009) , and the review in Huston (2010) . Related surveys in other countries examined similar financial literacy concepts (see, the Dutch Central Bank Household Survey, which has investigated and tested measures of financial literacy and financial sophistication, Alessie, Van Rooij, and Lusardi 2011 ).
Similar findings are reported for smaller samples or specific population subgroups (see Agnew and Szykman 2011 ; Utkus and Young 2011 ).
The Central Bank of Austria has used these questions to measure financial literacy in ten countries in Eastern Europe and we report the findings for Romania, where financial literacy has been studied in detail ( Beckman, 2013 ). These questions have also been fielded in Mexico and Chile ( Hastings and Tejeda-Ashton 2008 ; Hastings and Mitchell 2011 ; Behrman, Mitchell, Soo and Bravo 2012 ), India and Indonesia ( Cole, Sampson, and Zia 2011 ). They have also been used to measure financial literacy among Sri Lankan entrepreneurs ( de Mel, McKenzie, and Woodruff 2008 ) and a sample of U.S.-based migrants from El Salvador ( Ashraf, Aycinena, Martinez, and Yang 2011 ). We do not report the estimates for these countries because they do not always work with representative samples of the population or use samples that can be compared with the statistics reported in Table 2 .
Researchers have also examined answers to questions on mathematical numeracy in the England Longitudinal Survey of Ageing (ELSA; Banks and Oldfield 2007 ), and in the Survey of Health, Ageing, and Retirement in Europe (SHARE; Christelis, Jappelli, and Padula 2010 ).
Their survey uses eight financial literacy questions and focuses on fundamental concepts including the three main concepts discussed earlier.
For more information on the Financial Literacy Framework in PISA, see: http://www.oecd.org/pisa/pisaproducts/46962580.pdf
In the 2008 HRS, the financial literacy questions were modified to assess the sensitivity of peoples’ answers to the way in which the questions were worded. Results confirmed sensitivity to question wording, especially for the more sophisticated financial concepts ( Lusardi, Mitchell, and Curto 2012 ). Behrman, Mitchell, Soo and Bravo (2012) developed a financial literacy index employing a two-step weighting approach, whereby the first step weighted each question by difficulty and the second step applied principal components analysis to take into account correlations across questions. Resulting scores indicated how financially literate each individual was in relation to the average and to specific questions asked. The results confirmed that the basic financial literacy questions designed by Lusardi and Mitchell (2011b ) receive the largest weights.
Earlier we made mention of the widespread lack of financial and economic knowledge among high school and college students. At the other end of the work life, financial literacy also declines with age, as found in the 2004 HRS module on financial literacy on people age 50+ and in many other countries ( Lusardi and Mitchell 2011b , c ).
While statistics are only reported for four countries in Figure 1b , the prevalence of “do not know” responses by women is found in all of the twelve countries listed in Table 2 .
It may be possible but untested so far that women, for example young ones, expect they would have someone later in life (a husband or companion) to take care of their finances.
This might also help account for the sex differences mentioned above, since in many cultures, men are more likely than women to interact daily with financially knowledgeable individuals.
Other studies discussing financial socialization of the young include Hira, Sabri, and Loibl (2013) and the references cited therein.
For a review of the role of financial literacy in the consumer behavior literature, see Hira (2010) .
In 2011 Americans submitted over 1.5 million complaints about financial and other fraud, up 62 percent in just three years; these counts are also likely understatements ( FTC 2012 ). Financial losses per capita due to fraud have also increased over time: the median loss per victim rose from $218 in 2002 to $537 in 2011. Similarly the SEC (2012) warns about scams and fraud and other potential consequences of very low financial literacy, particularly among the most vulnerable groups.
The link between financial literacy and retirement planning also robust to the measure of financial literacy used (basic versus sophisticated financial knowledge; Lusardi and Mitchell 2009 , 2011d ), how planning is measured ( Lusardi and Mitchell 2007a , 2009a , 2011b ; Alessie, van Rooij, and Lusardi 2011 ), and which controls are included in the empirical estimation ( van Rooij, Lusardi, and Alessie 2011 ).
The alternative financial services (AFS) industry has experienced tremendous growth in the United States: in 2009, the Federal Deposit Insurance Corporation estimated the industry to be worth at least $320 billion in terms of transactional services ( FDIC 2009 ).
Disney and Gathergood (2012) reported that UK consumer credit customers systematically underestimated the cost of borrowing, while the least financially literate had higher average debt-to-income ratios.
Americans paid about $8 billion in finance charges to borrow more than $50 billion from payday lenders in 2007; the annual interest rates on such loans are often very high, over 400%. See Bertrand and Morse (2011) and the references therein.
See for instance Laibson (2011) .
See for instance Ameriks, Caplin, and Leahy (2003) ; van Rooij, Lusardi, and Alessie (2012) ; and Lusardi and Mitchell (2007a , b ; 2009 ). It is worth noting that education also plays a role, as pointed out by Poterba, Venti, and Wise (2013) who find a substantial association between education and the post-retirement evolution of assets. For example, for two-person households, assets growth between 1998 and 2008 was greater for college graduates than for those with less than a high school degree, producing over $600,000 in assets for the richest quintile, to $82,000 for the lowest asset quintile. As in the theoretical model described previously, households with different levels of education will invest in different assets, allowing them to earn different rates of return. It remains to be seen whether this is because of differential financial literacy investments, or simply due to general knowledge gleaned through education.
Gustman, Steinmeier, and Tabatabai (2010) note that financial knowledge is not the same thing as cognitive functioning, since the latter is not associated with greater knowledge of retirement plan rules.
Several authors have also linked financial literacy and knowledge about retirement saving. For instance, Agnew, Szykman, Utkus, and Young (2007) show that employees who were the least financially knowledgeable were 34 percent less likely to participate voluntarily, and 11 percent less likely to be automatically enrolled, in their in their company’s 401(k) plan.
These findings are not attributable to differences in individuals’ subjective life expectancies, discount rates, risk aversion, borrowing constraints, political risk, or other conventional explanations ( Brown, Kapteyn, Luttmer, and Mitchell 2011 ).
These include credit card balance transfers; home equity loans and lines of credit; auto loans; credit card interest rates; mortgages; small business credit cards; credit card late-payment fees; credit card over-limit fees; and credit card cash-advance fees.
This could be particularly important inasmuch as Korniotis and Kumar (2011) find that cognitive decline is fastest with age for the less educated, lower earners, and minority racial/ethnic groups.
Part of this behavior could also be due to liquidity constraints.
See for instance Collins and O’Rourke (2010) ; Gale, Harris and Levine (2012) ; Hastings, Madrian, and Skimmyhorn (2012) ; Hathaway and Khatiwada (2008) ; Lusardi and Mitchell (2007b ); Lyons, Palmer, Jayaratne, and Scherpf (2006) ; and Martin (2007) . Hira (2010) provides a broad overview of research on financial education over a long time span.
Two good discussions by Fox, Bartholomae, and Lee (2005) and Lyons and Neelakantan (2008) highlight the limitations of existing financial education program evaluations.
Moreover, practitioner discussions often refer to ‘financial capability,’ a term often identified with behavior change rather than knowledge.
The difference in the knowledge of risk diversification, tax benefits of retirement accounts, and the benefits of employers’ matches between the two groups (measured by the proportion of correct answers) was on the order of 10 percentage points. While these videos were targeted to young adults, older respondents who viewed them also increased knowledge and capacity to correctly answer questions concerning saving decisions ( Heinberg, Hung, Kapteyn, Lusardi, and Yoong, 2010 ).
A discussion of successful strategies to improve financial literacy and financial education programs is provided in Crossan (2011)
For as broad perspective on how financial education programs can be made more effective in developing countries see Holzmann (2011) .
Among other things, the Bureau’s mandate is to promote financial education and monitor financial markets for new risks to consumers; see http://www.consumerfinance.gov/the-bureau/ .
For instance, the Director of the Consumer Financial Protection Bureau, Richard Cordray, has been a strong supporter of financial education in high school and in the workplace.
Note, however, that when left to their own devices, many employees simply fail to enroll in pensions and hence fail to exploit the employer match at all , if or when one is available.
A detailed analysis of the issues surrounding financial advice appears in Mitchell and Smetters (2013) .
Contributor Information
Annamaria Lusardi, The George Washington University School of Business, Duques Hall, Suite 450E, Washington, DC 20052, Tel: (202) 994-8410.
Olivia S. Mitchell, Department of Insurance & Risk Management, The Wharton School, Univ. of Pennsylvania, 3620 Locust Walk, St. 3000 SH-DH, Philadelphia, PA 19104, Tel: (215) 898-0424
- Agarwal Sumit, Amromin Gene, Ben-David Itzhak, Chomsisengphet Souphala, Evanoff Douglas. Financial Counseling, Financial Literacy, and Household Decisionmaking. In: Mitchell OS, Lusardi A, editors. Financial Literacy: Implications for Retirement Security and the Financial Marketplace. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2011. pp. 181–205. [ Google Scholar ]
- Agarwal Sumit, Driscoll John, Gabaix Xavier, Laibson David. The Age of Reason: Financial Decisions over the Lifecycle with Implications for Regulation. Brookings Papers on Economic Activity. 2009 Fall;:51–101. [ Google Scholar ]
- Agarwal Sumit, Skiba Paige, Tobacman Jeremy. (Wharton School Working Paper, University of Pennsylvania).Payday Loans and Credit Cards: New Liquidity and Credit Scoring Puzzles? 2009 [ Google Scholar ]
- Agnew Julie, Bateman Hazel, Thorp Susan. Financial Literacy and Retirement Planning in Australia. University of New South Wales; 2013. (Working Paper). forthcoming Numeracy. [ Google Scholar ]
- Agnew Julie, Szykman Lisa. Annuities, Financial Literacy and Information Overload. In: Mitchell OS, Lusardi A, editors. Financial Literacy: Implications for Retirement Security and the Financial Marketplace. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2011. pp. 158–178. [ Google Scholar ]
- Agnew Julie, Szykman Lisa, Utkus Stephen, Young Jean. Literacy, Trust and 401(k) Savings Behavior. Boston College; 2007. (Center for Retirement Research Working Paper 2007–10). [ Google Scholar ]
- Alessie Rob, van Rooij Maarten, Lusardi Annamaria. Financial Literacy and Retirement Preparation in the Netherlands. Journal of Pension Economics and Finance. 2011;10(4):527–545. [ Google Scholar ]
- Allgood Sam, Walstad William. Financial Literacy and Credit Card Behaviors: A Cross-Section Analysis by Age. 2013 Forthcoming Numeracy. [ Google Scholar ]
- Almenberg Johann, Dreber Anna. (Stockholm School of Economics Working Paper).Gender, Financial Literacy, and Stock Market Participation. 2011 [ Google Scholar ]
- Ameriks John, Caplin Andrew, Leahy John. Wealth Accumulation and the Propensity to Plan. Quarterly Journal of Economics. 2003;118:1007–1047. [ Google Scholar ]
- Arenas de Mesa Alberto, Bravo David, Behrman Jere R, Mitchell Olivia S, Todd Petra E. The Chilean Pension Reform Turns 25: Lessons from the Social Protection Survey. In: Kay S, Sinha T, editors. Lessons from Pension Reform in the Americas. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2008. pp. 23–58. [ Google Scholar ]
- Arrondel Luc, Debbich Majdi, Savignac Frédérique. Stockholding and Financial Literacy in the French Population. International Journal of Social Sciences and Humanity Studies. 2012;4(2):1309–8063. [ Google Scholar ]
- Arrondel Luc, Debbich Majdi, Savignac Frédérique. Financial Literacy and Planning in France. 2013 Forthcoming Numeracy. [ Google Scholar ]
- Ashraf Nava, Aycinena Diego, Claudia Martinez A, Yang Dean. Remittances and the Problem of Control: A Field Experiment Among Migrants from El Salvador. University of Michigan; 2011. (Working Paper). [ Google Scholar ]
- Atkinson Adele, Flore-Anne Messy. Assessing Financial Literacy in 12 Countries: An OECD/INFE International Pilot Exercise. Journal of Pension Economics and Finance. 2011;10(4):657–665. [ Google Scholar ]
- Atkinson Adele, Messy Flore-Anne. Measuring Financial Literacy: Results of the OECD/International Network on Financial Education (INFE) Pilot Study. OECD; 2012. [ Google Scholar ]
- Attanasio Orazio, Guglielmo Weber. Consumption and Saving: Models of Intertemporal Allocation and Their Implications for Public Policy. Journal of Economic Literature. 2010;48:693–751. [ Google Scholar ]
- Banks James, Oldfield Zoe. Understanding Pensions: Cognitive Functions, Numerical Ability and Retirement Saving. Fiscal Studies. 2007;28(2) [ Google Scholar ]
- Becker Gary S. Human Capital: A Theoretical and Empirical Analysis. 2nd. New York: Columbia University Press; 1975. [ Google Scholar ]
- Beckmann Elisabeth. Financial Literacy and Household Savings in Romania. 2013 Jul; Forthcoming Numeracy. 2013. [ Google Scholar ]
- Behrman Jere, Mitchell Olivia S, Soo Cindy, Bravo David. Financial Literacy, Schooling, and Wealth Accumulation. American Economic Review. 2012;102(3):300–304. doi: 10.1257/aer.102.3.300. [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Benítez-Silva Hugo, Demiralp Berna, Liu Zhen. (MRRC Working Paper 2009-210).Social Security Literacy and Retirement Well-Being. 2009 [ Google Scholar ]
- Ben-Porath Yoram. The Production of Human Capital and the Life Cycle of Earnings. Journal of Political Economy. 1967;75(4):352–365. [ Google Scholar ]
- Bernanke Ben S. Statement of the Chairman of the Board of Governors of the Federal Reserve System at a Hearing conducted by the Subcommittee on Oversight of Government Management, the Federal Workforce, and the District of Columbia of the Committee on Homeland Security and Governmental Affairs, US Senate. Washington, DC: 2011. Apr 12, [ Google Scholar ]
- Bernheim Douglas. Tax Policy and Economic Growth. Washington, DC: American Council for Capital Formation; 1995. Do Households Appreciate their Financial Vulnerabilities? An Analysis of Actions, Perceptions, and Public Policy; pp. 1–30. [ Google Scholar ]
- Bernheim Douglas. Financial Illiteracy, Education, and Retirement Saving. In: Mitchell OS, Schieber S, editors. Living with Defined Contribution Pensions. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press; 1998. pp. 38–68. [ Google Scholar ]
- Bernheim Douglas, Daniel Garrett Dean Maki. Education and Saving: The Long-term Effects of High School Financial Curriculum Mandates. Journal of Public Economics. 2001;80:435–565. [ Google Scholar ]
- Bernheim Douglas, Garrett Daniel. The Effects of Financial Education in the Workplace: Evidence from a Survey of Households. Journal of Public Economics. 2003;87:1487–1519. [ Google Scholar ]
- Bernheim Douglas, Scholz John Karl. Private Saving and Public Policy. In: Poterba James., editor. Tax Policy and the Economy. Vol. 7. MIT Press; 1993. pp. 73–110. [ Google Scholar ]
- Bertrand Marianne, Morse Adair. Information Disclosure, Cognitive Biases, and Payday Borrowing. Journal of Finance. 2011;66(6):1865–1893. [ Google Scholar ]
- Beshears John, Choi James, Laibson David, Madrian Brigitte. (RAND Working Paper).Default Stickiness among Low-Income Individuals. 2012 [ Google Scholar ]
- Blanton Kimberly. Center for Retirement Research brief. Boston, Mass: Boston College; 2012. The Rise of Financial Fraud. [ Google Scholar ]
- Brown Jeffrey R, Kapteyn Arie, Mitchell Olivia S. Framing and Claiming: How Information-Framing Affects Expected Social Security Claiming Behavior. Forthcoming Journal of Risk and Insurance. 2013 doi: 10.1111/j.1539-6975.2013.12004.x. [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Brown Jeffrey, Kapteyn Arie, Luttmer Erzo, Mitchell Olivia S. Do Consumers Know How to Value Annuities? (RAND Working Paper WR-924-SSA).Complexity as a Barrier to Annuitization. 2011 Sep [ Google Scholar ]
- Brown Martin, Graf Roman. Financial Literacy and Retirement Planning in Switzerland. University of S. Gallen; 2013. Jul, (Swiss Institute of Banking and Finance Working Paper). Forthcoming Numeracy. 2013. [ Google Scholar ]
- Browning Martin, Lusardi Annamaria. Household Saving: Micro Theories and Micro Facts. Journal of Economic Literature. 1996;34:1797–1855. [ Google Scholar ]
- Bruhn Miriam, Legovini Arianna, Zia Bilal. (World Bank Working Paper).Financial Literacy for High School Students and Their Parents: Evidence from Brazil. 2012 [ Google Scholar ]
- Bucher-Koenen Tabea, Lusardi Annamaria. Financial Literacy and Retirement Planning in Germany. Journal of Pension Economics and Finance. 2011;10(4):565–584. doi: 10.1017/S1474747211000448. [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Bucher-Koenen Tabea, Ziegelmeyer Michael. (MEA Working Paper N. 234).Who Lost the Most? Financial Literacy, Cognitive Abilities and the Financial Crisis. 2011 [ Google Scholar ]
- Bucher-Koenen Tabea, Lusardi Annamaria, Alessie Rob, van Rooij Martin. (Netspar Working Paper N. 31).How Financially Literate are Women? Some New Perspectives on the Gender Gap. 2012 [ Google Scholar ]
- Bumcrot Chris, Lin Judy, Lusardi Annamaria. (Rand Working Paper N. WR-893-SS).The Geography of Financial Literacy. 2013 Forthcoming Numeracy. [ Google Scholar ]
- Bursztyn Leonardo, Ederer Florian, Ferman Bruno, Yuchtman Noam. (Working Paper, The George Washington School of Business).Understanding Peer Influence in Financial Decisions: Evidence from a Field Experiment. 2013 [ Google Scholar ]
- Cagetti Marco. Wealth Accumulation over the Life Cycle and Precautionary Savings. Journal of Business and Economic Statistics. 2003;21(3):339–353. [ Google Scholar ]
- Cai Jing, de Janvry Alain, Sadoulet Elisabeth. (Working Paper, University of Michigan).Social Networks and the Decision to Insure. 2013 [ Google Scholar ]
- Calvet Laurent, Campbell John, Sodini Paolo. Down or Out: Assessing the Welfare Costs of Household Investment Mistakes. Journal of Political Economy. 2007;115:707–747. [ Google Scholar ]
- Calvet Laurent, Campbell John, Sodini Paolo. Measuring the Financial Sophistication of Households. American Economic Review. 2009;99:393–398. [ Google Scholar ]
- Campbell John. Household Finance. Journal of Finance. 2006;61:1553–1604. [ Google Scholar ]
- Campbell John, Jackson Howell, Madrian Brigitte, Tufano Peter. Consumer Financial Protection. Journal of Economic Perspectives. 2011;25(1):91–114. doi: 10.1257/jep.25.1.91. [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Carpena Fenella, Cole Shawn, Shapiro Jeremy, Zia Bilal. (World Bank Policy Research Working Paper 5798).Unpacking the Causal Chain of Financial Literacy. 2011 [ Google Scholar ]
- Center for Responsible Lending. Center Report. Durham: NC: 2004. Quantifying the Economic Cost of Predatory Pay-day Lending. [ Google Scholar ]
- Chai Jingjing, Horneff Wolfram, Maurer Raimond, Mitchell Olivia S. Optimal Portfolio Choice over the Life Cycle with Flexible Work, Endogenous Retirement, and Lifetime Payouts. Review of Finance. 2012;15(4):875–907. [ Google Scholar ]
- Chan Sewin, Stevens Anne. What You Don’t Know Cannot Help You: Pension Knowledge and Retirement Decision-making. Review of Economics and Statistics. 2008;90:253–266. [ Google Scholar ]
- Chen Haiyang, Volpe Ronald P. An Analysis of Personal Financial Literacy among College Students. Financial Services Review. 1998;7:107–128. [ Google Scholar ]
- Chen Haiyang, Volpe Ronald P. Gender Differences in Personal Financial Literacy among College Students. Financial Services Review. 2002;11(3):289. [ Google Scholar ]
- Chiteji Ngina, Frank Stafford. Portfolio Choices of Parents and their Children as Young Adults: Asset Accumulation by African American Families. American Economic Review. 1999;89:377–380. [ Google Scholar ]
- Christelis Dimitris, Jappelli Tullio, Padula Mario. Cognitive Abilities and Portfolio Choice. European Economic Review. 2010;54:18–39. [ Google Scholar ]
- Christiansen Charlotte, Joensen Juanna, Rangvid Jesper. Are Economists More Likely to Hold Stocks? Review of Finance. 2008;12:465–496. [ Google Scholar ]
- Choi James, Laibson David, Madrian Brigitte C. Plan Design and 401(k) Savings Outcomes. National Tax Journal. 2004;57:275–298. [ Google Scholar ]
- Choi James, Laibson David, Madrian Brigitte C, Metrick Andrew. Saving for Retirement on the Path of Least Resistance. In: McCaffrey Edward J, Slemrod Joel., editors. Behavioral Public Finance: Toward a New Agenda. New York: Russell Sage Foundation; 2006. pp. 304–351. [ Google Scholar ]
- Clark Robert, D’Ambrosio Madeleine. Adjusting Retirement Goals and Saving Behavior: The Role of Financial Education. In: Lusardi A, editor. Overcoming the Saving Slump: How to Increase the Effectiveness of Financial Education and Saving Programs. Chicago: University of Chicago Press; 2008. pp. 237–256. [ Google Scholar ]
- Clark Robert, Morrill Melinda, Allen Steven. Effectiveness of Employer-Provided Financial Information: Hiring to Retiring. American Economic Review. 2012a May;:314–318. [ Google Scholar ]
- Clark Robert, Morrill Melinda, Allen Steven. The Role of Financial Literacy in Determining Retirement Plans. Economic Inquiry. 2012b;50(4):851–866. [ Google Scholar ]
- Clark Robert, Ogawa Naohiro, Matsukura Rikiya. (Population Dynamics and Economic Development Working Paper).Low Fertility, Human Capital, and Economic Growth: The Importance of Financial Education and Job Retraining. 2010 [ Google Scholar ]
- Cocco Joao, Gomes Francisco, Maenhout Pascal. Consumption and Portfolio Choice over the Life-cycle. Review of Financial Studies. 2005;18:490–533. [ Google Scholar ]
- Cole Shawn, Xavier Giné Jeremy Tobacman, Petia Topalova Robert Townsend, James Vickery. Barriers to Household Risk Management: Evidence from India. American Economic Journal: Applied Economics. 2013;5(1):104–135. doi: 10.1257/app.5.1.104. [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Cole Shawn, Sampson Thomas, Zia Bilal. Prices or Knowledge? What Drives Demand for Financial Services in Emerging Markets? Journal of Finance. 2011;66(6):1933–1967. [ Google Scholar ]
- Collins J Michael. Improving Financial Literacy: The Role of Nonprofit Providers. In: Mitchell OS, Lusardi A, editors. Financial Literacy: Implications for Retirement Security and the Financial Marketplace. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2011. pp. 268–187. [ Google Scholar ]
- Collins J Michael, O’Rourke Colin M. Financial Education and Counseling: Still Holding Promise. Journal of Consumer Affairs. 2010;44(3):483–489. [ Google Scholar ]
- Crossan Diana. How to Improve Financial Literacy: Some Successful Strategies. In: Mitchell OS, Lusardi A, editors. Financial Literacy: Implications for Retirement Security and the Financial Marketplace. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2011. pp. 241–254. [ Google Scholar ]
- de Bassa Scheresberg. Financial Literacy and Financial Behavior Among Young Adults Evidence and Implications. 2013 Forthcoming Numeracy. [ Google Scholar ]
- Deevy Martha, Lucich Shoshana, Beals Michaela. (Stanford Financial Fraud Research Center Working Paper).Scams, Schemes, and Swindles: A Review of Consumer Financial Fraud Research. 2012 [ Google Scholar ]
- Delavande Adeline, Rohwedder Susann, Willis Robert. (MRRC Working Paper Series 190).Preparation for Retirement, Financial Knowledge and Cognitive Resources. 2008 [ Google Scholar ]
- De Mel Suresh, McKenzie David, Woodruff Christopher. Getting Credit to High Return Microentrepreneurs: The Result of an Information Intervention. World Bank Economic Review. 2008:456–486. [ Google Scholar ]
- DeNardi Mariacristina, French Eric, Jones John Bailey. Why Do the Elderly Save? The Role of Medical Expenses. Journal of Political Economy. 2011;118(1):39–75. [ Google Scholar ]
- Disney Richard, Gathergood John. (Working Paper, University of Nottingham).Financial Literacy and Consumer Credit Portfolios. 2012 [ Google Scholar ]
- Drexel Alejandro, Fischer Greg, Schoar Antoinette. (Working Paper, MIT).Keeping it Simple: Financial Literacy and Rules of Thumb. 2011 [ Google Scholar ]
- Duflo Esther, Saez Emmanuel. The Role of Information and Social Interactions in Retirement Plan Decisions: Evidence from a Randomized Experiment. Quarterly Journal of Economics. 2003;118:815–842. [ Google Scholar ]
- Duflo Esther, Emmanuel Saez. Implications of Pension Plan Features, Information, and Social Interactions for Retirement Saving Decisions. In: Mitchell OS, Utkus S, editors. Pension Design and Structure: New Lessons from Behavioral Finance. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2004. pp. 137–153. [ Google Scholar ]
- Elliehausen Gregory, Lundquist Christopher, Staten Michael. The Impact of Credit Counseling on Subsequent Borrower Behavior. Journal of Consumer Affairs. 2007;41(1):1–28. [ Google Scholar ]
- Employee Benefit Retirement Institute. Retirement Confidence Survey. 2007 http://www.ebri.org/surveys/rcs/
- FDIC Unbanked/Underbanked Survey Study Group. FDIC National Survey of Unbanked and Underbanked Households. Washington, DC: Federal Deposit Insurance Corporation; 2009. [ Google Scholar ]
- Federal Trade Commission. Consumer Sentinel Network Data Book. Washington D.C: FTC; 2012. [ Google Scholar ]
- Financial Industry National Regulatory Authority (FINRA) Investor Literacy and Fraud Susceptibility Survey: Executive Summary. Washington, DC: FINRA; 2006. http://www.finra.org/Investors/ProtectYourself/AvoidInvestmentFraud/ [ Google Scholar ]
- FINRA Investor Education Foundation. Financial Capability in the United States. National Survey - Executive Summary. 2009 Dec; 2009. [ Google Scholar ]
- FINRA Investor Education Foundation. Financial Capability in the United States. Report of Findings from the 2012 National Financial Capability Study. 2013 May; 2013. [ Google Scholar ]
- Finke Michael. Financial Advice: Does It Make a Difference? In: Mitchell OS, Smetters K, editors. The Market for Retirement Financial Advice. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press; 2013. Forthcoming. [ Google Scholar ]
- Finke Michael, Howe John, Houston Sandra. Old Age and the Decline in Financial Literacy. SSRN; 2011. http://ssrn.com/abstract=1948627 . [ Google Scholar ]
- Fonseca Raquel, Mullen Kathleen J, Zamarro Gema, Zissimopoulos Julie. What explains the gender gap in financial literacy? The Role of Household Decision Making. Journal of Consumer Affairs. 2012 Spring;:90–106. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-6606.2011.01221.x. [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Fornero Elsa, Monticone Chiara. Financial Literacy and Pension Plan Participation in Italy. Journal of Pension Economics and Finance. 2011;10(4):547–564. [ Google Scholar ]
- Fox Jonathan, Bartholomae Suzanne, Lee Jinkook. Building the Case for Financial Education. Journal of Consumer Research. 2005;39(1):195–214. [ Google Scholar ]
- French Eric. The Effects of Health, Wealth, and Wages on Labor Supply and Retirement Behavior. Review of Economic Studies. 2005;72(2):395–427. [ Google Scholar ]
- French Kenneth. The Cost of Active Investing. Journal of Finance. 2008;63:1537–1573. [ Google Scholar ]
- Friedman Milton. A Theory of the Consumption Function. Princeton: Princeton University Press; 1957. [ Google Scholar ]
- Gale William, Harris Benjamin, Levine Ruth. Raising Household Saving. Does Financial Education Work? Social Security Bulletin. 2012 May; N. 72-2. 2012. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Gerardi Kristopher, Goette Lorenz, Meier Stephan. (Working paper and forthcoming in Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences).Numerical Ability Predicts Mortgage Default. 2013 doi: 10.1073/pnas.1220568110. [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Glewwe Paul. Schools and Skills in Developing Countries: Education Policies and Socioeconomic Outcomes. Journal of Economic Literature. 2002;40:436–482. [ Google Scholar ]
- Goda Shah Gopi, Manchester Colleen, Sojourner Aaron. What Will My Account Really be Worth? (RAND Working Paper WR-873).An Experiment on Exponential Growth Bias and Retirement Saving. 2012 [ Google Scholar ]
- Gourinchas Pierre-Olivier, Parker Jonathan. Consumption over the Life Cycle. Econometrica. 2002;70(1):47–89. [ Google Scholar ]
- Grinblatt Mark, Keloharju Matt, Linnainmaa Juhani. IQ and Stock Market Participation. Journal of Finance. 2011;66(6):2121–2164. [ Google Scholar ]
- Gustman Alan, Steinmeier Thomas, Tabatabai Nahid. (NBER Working Paper 16500).Financial Knowledge and Financial Literacy at the Household Level. 2010 [ Google Scholar ]
- Haliassos Michael, Bertaut Carol. Why Do So Few Hold Stocks? Economic Journal. 1995;105:1110–1129. [ Google Scholar ]
- Hanushek Eric, Woessmann Ludger. The Role of Cognitive Skills in Economic Development. Journal of Economic Literature. 2008;46(3):607–668. [ Google Scholar ]
- Hastings Justine, Madrian Brigitte, Skimmyhorn William. (NBER Working Paper 18412).Financial Literacy, Financial Education and Economic Outcomes. 2012 doi: 10.1146/annurev-economics-082312-125807. [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Hastings Justine, Mitchell Olivia S. (NBER Working Paper 16740).How Financial Literacy and Impatience Shape Retirement Wealth and Investment Behaviors. 2011 doi: 10.1017/s1474747218000227. [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Hastings Justine, Mitchell Olivia S, Chyn Eric. Fees, Framing, and Financial Literacy in the Choice of Pension Managers. In: Mitchell OS, Lusardi A, editors. Financial Literacy: Implications for Retirement Security and the Financial Marketplace. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2011. pp. 101–115. [ Google Scholar ]
- Hastings Justine, Tejeda-Ashton Lydia. (NBER Working Paper 14538).Financial Literacy, Information, and Demand Elasticity: Survey and Experimental Evidence from Mexico. 2008 [ Google Scholar ]
- Hathaway Ian, Khatiwada Sameer. (Federal Reserve Bank of Cleveland Working Paper).Do Financial Education Programs Work? 2008 [ Google Scholar ]
- Heinberg Aileen, Hung Angela, Kapteyn Arie, Lusardi Annamaria, Yoong Joanne. (RAND Working Paper No. WR-790-SSA).Five Steps to Planning Success. 2010 2010. [ Google Scholar ]
- Hilgert Marianne, Hogarth Jeanne, Beverly Sondra. Household Financial Management: The Connection between Knowledge and Behavior. Federal Reserve Bulletin. 2003:309–322. [ Google Scholar ]
- Hira Tahira. (National Endowment for Financial Education Working Paper).The NEFE Quarter Century Project: Implications for Researchers, Educators and Policy Makers from a Quarter Century of Financial Education. 2010 http://www.nefe.org/Portals/0/WhatWeProvide/PrimaryResearch/PDF/TheQtrCenturyProject_FinalSeminal.pdf .
- Hira Tahira, Mohamed Sabri, Cäzilia Loibl. Financial Socialization’s Impact on Investment Orientation and Household Net Worth. International Journal of Consumer Studies. 2013;37(1):29–35. [ Google Scholar ]
- Hirad Abdighani, Zorn Peter. A Little Knowledge is a Good Thing: Empirical Evidence of the Effectiveness of Pre-Purchase Homeownership Counseling. In: Retsinas Nicolas, Belsky Eric., editors. Low-income Homeownership: Examining the Unexamined Goal. Cambridge, Mass & Washington, D.C.: Joint Center for Housing Studies; Brookings Institution Press; 2002. pp. 146–174. [ Google Scholar ]
- Holzmann Robert. Bringing Financial Literacy and Education to Low and Middle Income Countries. In: Mitchell OS, Lusardi A, editors. Financial Literacy: Implications for Retirement Security and the Financial Marketplace. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2011. pp. 255–267. [ Google Scholar ]
- Huston Sandra. Measuring Financial Literacy. Journal of Consumer Affairs. 2010;44:296–316. [ Google Scholar ]
- Hsu Joanne. (Networks Financial Institute Working Paper 2011-WP-06, Indiana State University).Aging and Strategic Learning: The Impact of Spousal Incentives on Financial Literacy. 2011 doi: 10.3368/jhr.51.4.1014-6712r. [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Hung Angela, Parker Andrew, Yoong Joanne. (RAND Working Paper 708).Defining and Measuring Financial Literacy. 2009 [ Google Scholar ]
- Hurst Eric, Aguiar Mark. Consumption vs Expenditure. Journal of Political Economy. 2005;113(5):919–948. [ Google Scholar ]
- Hurst Eric, Mark Aguiar. Measuring Trends in Leisure: The Allocation of Time Over Five Decades. Quarterly Journal of Economics. 2007;122(3):969–1006. [ Google Scholar ]
- Jappelli Tullio. Economic Literacy: An International Comparison. The Economic Journal. 2010;120:429–451. [ Google Scholar ]
- Jappelli Tullio, Padula Mario. Investment in Financial Literacy and Saving Decisions. Journal of Banking and Finance. 2013;37:2779–2792. [ Google Scholar ]
- Kast Felipe, Meier Stephan, Pomeranz Dina. (HBS Working Paper 12-060).Under-Savers Anonymous: Evidence on Self-Help Groups and Peer Pressure as a Savings Commitment Device. 2012 [ Google Scholar ]
- Kézdi Gabor, Willis Robert. (NBER WP 17614).Household Stock Market Beliefs and Learning. 2011 [ Google Scholar ]
- Kimball Miles, Shumway Tyler. (University of Michigan Working Paper).Investor Sophistication and the Participation, Home Bias, Diversification, and Employer Stock Puzzles. 2006 [ Google Scholar ]
- Klapper Leora, Panos Georgios. Financial Literacy and Retirement Planning: The Russian Case. Journal of Pension Economics and Finance. 2011;10(4):599–618. [ Google Scholar ]
- Klapper Leora, Lusardi Annamaria, Panos Georgios. (NBER Working Paper 17930).Financial Literacy and the Financial Crisis. 2012 [ Google Scholar ]
- Korniotis George, Alok Kumar. Do Older Investors Make Better Investment Decisions? Review of Economics and Statistics. 2011;93(4):244–265. [ Google Scholar ]
- Laibson David. The Age of Reason Presentation for the Department of Economics. Harvard University; 2011. http://www.economics.harvard.edu/faculty/laibson/files/Age%2Bof%2BReason.pdf . [ Google Scholar ]
- Li Geng. (Federal Reserve Board Working Paper).Information Sharing and Stock Market Participation: Evidence from Extended Families. 2009 [ Google Scholar ]
- Lu Timothy, Mitchell Olivia S, Utkus Stephen P. (MRRC Working Paper WR-799-SSA).An Empirical Analysis of 401(k) Loan Defaults. 2010 [ Google Scholar ]
- Lusardi Annamaria. (NBER Working Paper 17103).Americans’ Financial Capability. 2011 [ Google Scholar ]
- Lusardi Annamaria, Scheresberg Carlo de Bassa. (NBER Working Paper 18969).Financial Literacy and High-Cost Borrowing in the United States. 2013 [ Google Scholar ]
- Lusardi Annamaria, Keller Punam, Keller Adam. New Ways to Make People Save: A Social Marketing Approach. In: Lusardi A, editor. Overcoming the Saving Slump: How to Increase the Effectiveness of Financial Education and Saving Programs. Chicago: University of Chicago Press; 2008. pp. 209–236. [ Google Scholar ]
- Lusardi Annamaria, Michaud Pierre-Carl, Mitchell Olivia S. (Wharton School Pension Research Council Working Paper WP2011-20).Optimal Financial Literacy and Saving for Retirement. 2011 [ Google Scholar ]
- Lusardi Annamaria, Michaud Pierre-Carl, Mitchell Olivia S. (NBER Working Paper 18669).Optimal Financial Literacy and Wealth Inequality. 2013 [ Google Scholar ]
- Lusardi Annamaria, Mitchell Olivia S. Baby Boomers’ Retirement Security: The Role of Planning, Financial Literacy and Housing Wealth. Journal of Monetary Economics. 2007a;54:205–224. [ Google Scholar ]
- Lusardi Annamaria, Mitchell Olivia S. Financial Literacy and Retirement Preparedness: Evidence and Implications for Financial Education. Business Economics. 2007b:35–44. [ Google Scholar ]
- Lusardi Annamaria, Mitchell Olivia S. Planning and Financial Literacy: How Do Women Fare? American Economic Review. 2008;98:413–417. [ Google Scholar ]
- Lusardi Annamaria, Mitchell Olivia S. (NBER Working Paper 15350).How Ordinary Consumers Make Complex Economic Decisions: Financial Literacy and Retirement Readiness. 2009 [ Google Scholar ]
- Lusardi Annamaria, Mitchell Olivia S. The Outlook for Financial Literacy. In: Mitchell OS, Lusardi A, editors. Financial Literacy: Implications for Retirement Security and the Financial Marketplace. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2011a. pp. 1–15. [ Google Scholar ]
- Lusardi Annamaria, Mitchell Olivia S. Financial Literacy and Planning: Implications for Retirement Wellbeing. In: Mitchell OS, Lusardi A, editors. Financial Literacy: Implications for Retirement Security and the Financial Marketplace. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2011b. pp. 17–39. [ Google Scholar ]
- Lusardi Annamaria, Mitchell Olivia S. Financial Literacy around the World: An Overview. Journal of Pension Economics and Finance. 2011c;10(4):497–508. doi: 10.1017/S1474747211000448. [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Lusardi Annamaria, Mitchell Olivia S. Financial Literacy and Retirement Planning in the United States. Journal of Pension Economics and Finance. 2011d;10(4):509–525. doi: 10.1017/S1474747211000448. [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Lusardi Annamaria, Mitchell Olivia S, Curto Vilsa. Financial Literacy Among the Young. Journal of Consumer Affairs. 2010;44(2):358–380. [ Google Scholar ]
- Lusardi Annamaria, Mitchell Olivia S, Curto Vilsa. (NBER Working Paper 17863).Financial Sophistication in the Older Population. 2012 doi: 10.1017/S1474747214000031. [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Lusardi Annamaria, Schneider Daniel J, Tufano Peter. Financially Fragile Households: Evidence and Implications. Brookings Papers on Economic Activity. 2011 Spring;:83–134. [ Google Scholar ]
- Lusardi Annamaria, Tufano Peter. (NBER Working Paper 14808).Debt Literacy, Financial Experiences, and Overindebtedness. 2009a [ Google Scholar ]
- Lusardi Annamaria, Tufano Peter. Teach Workers about the Peril of Debt. Harvard Business Review. 2009b Nov;:22–24. [ Google Scholar ]
- Lyons Angela, Palmer Lance, Jayaratne Korallalage, Scherpf Eric. Are We Making the Grade? A National Overview of Financial Education and Program Evaluation. Journal of Consumer Affairs. 2006;40(2):208–235. [ Google Scholar ]
- Lyons Angela, Neelakantan Urvi. Potential and Pitfalls of Applying Theory to the Practice of Financial Education. Journal of Consumer Affairs. 2008;42(1):106–112. [ Google Scholar ]
- Mahdavi Mahnaz, Horton Nicholas. (Working Paper, Smith College).Financial Literacy among Educated Women: Room for Improvement. 2012 [ Google Scholar ]
- Mandell Lewis. Our Vulnerable Youth: The Financial Literacy of American 12th Graders. Jump$tart Coalition for Personal Financial Literacy 1997 [ Google Scholar ]
- Mandell Lewis. Financial Literacy: Are We Improving? Washington, D.C.: Jump$tart Coalition for Personal Financial Literacy; 2004. [ Google Scholar ]
- Mandell Lewis. Financial Education in High School. In: Lusardi A, editor. Overcoming the Saving Slump: How to Increase the Effectiveness of Financial Education and Saving Programs. Chicago: University of Chicago Press; 2008. pp. 257–279. [ Google Scholar ]
- Madrian Brigitte, Shea Dennis. The Power of Suggestion: Inertia in 401(k) Participation and Saving Behavior. Quarterly Journal of Economics. 2001;116(4):1149–1187. [ Google Scholar ]
- Markow Dana, Bagnaschi Kelly. What American Teens & Adults Know About Economics. National Council on Economic Education Report 2005 [ Google Scholar ]
- Martin Matthew. (Federal Reserve Bank of Richmond, Working Paper 07-03).A Literature Review on the Effectiveness of Financial Education. 2007 [ Google Scholar ]
- McArdle James, Smith James, Willis Robert. Cognition and Economic Outcomes in the Health and Retirement Survey NBER Working Paper 15266 2009 [ Google Scholar ]
- Meier Steven, Sprenger Charles. (Columbia Business School Working Paper).Discounting Financial Literacy: Time Preferences and Participation in Financial Education Programs. 2008 [ Google Scholar ]
- Mitchell Olivia S, Piggott John, Takayama Noriyuki., editors. Securing Lifelong Retirement Income: Market Developments and Policy Issues. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2011. [ Google Scholar ]
- Mitchell Olivia S, Smetters Kent. The Market for Retirement Financial Advice. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2013. Forthcoming. [ Google Scholar ]
- Mitchell Olivia S, Utkus Stephen. Target-Date Funds in 401(k) Retirement Plans NBER Working Paper No 17911 2012 [ Google Scholar ]
- Modigliani Franco, Brumberg Richard. Utility Analysis and the Consumption Function: An Interpretation of Cross-section Data. In: Kurihara K, editor. Post-Keynesian Economics. New Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers University Press; 1954. pp. 388–436. [ Google Scholar ]
- Moore Danna. Survey of Financial Literacy in Washington State: Knowledge, Behavior, Attitudes and Experiences. Washington State University Social and Economic Sciences Research Center Technical Report 03-39 2003 [ Google Scholar ]
- Mottola Gary. In Our Best: Interest: Women, Financial Literacy, and Credit Card Behavior. Insights, FINRA Investor Education Foundation. 2013 Forthcoming Numeracy. [ Google Scholar ]
- Organization for Economic Co-Operation and Development (OECD) Improving Financial Literacy: Analysis of Issues and Policies. Paris, France: OECD; 2005. [ Google Scholar ]
- Poterba James, Venti Steven, Wise David. The Changing Landscape of Pensions in the United States. In: Lusardi A, editor. Overcoming the Saving Slump: How to Increase the Effectiveness of Financial Education and Saving Programs. Chicago: University of Chicago Press; 2008. pp. 17–46. [ Google Scholar ]
- Poterba James, Venti Steven, Wise David. (NBER Working Paper 18695).Health, Education, and the Post-Retirement Evolution of Household Assets. 2013 doi: 10.1086/673207. [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- President’s Advisory Committee on Financial Literacy (PACFL) Annual Report to the President: Executive Summary 2008 [ Google Scholar ]
- Romagnoli Angela, Trifilidis Maurizio. (Bank of Italy Working Paper).Does Financial Education in School Work? Evidence from Italy. 2012 [ Google Scholar ]
- Scholz John Karl, Seshadri Ananth, Khitatrakun Surachai. Are Americans Saving “Optimally” For Retirement? Journal of Political Economy. 2006;114:607–643. [ Google Scholar ]
- Sekita Shizuka. Financial Literacy and Retirement Planning in Japan. Journal of Pension Economics and Finance. 2011;10(4):637–656. doi: 10.1017/S1474747211000448. [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Shim Soyeon, Xiao Jing, Barber Bonnie, Lyons Angela. Pathway to Life Success: A Conceptual Model of Financial Well-being for Young Adults. Journal of Applied Development Psychology. 2009;30:708–723. [ Google Scholar ]
- Shim Soyeon, Barber Bonnie, Card Noel, Jing Xiao, Joyce Serido. Financial Socialization of First-year College Students: The Role of Parents, Work, and Education. Journal of Youth & Adolescence. 2010;39(12):1457–1470. doi: 10.1007/s10964-009-9432-x. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Skinner Jonathan. Are You Sure You’re Saving Enough for Retirement? Journal of Economic Perspectives. 2007 Summer;:59–80. [ Google Scholar ]
- Song Changcheng. (University of California, Berkeley Working Paper).Financial Illiteracy and Pension Contributions: A Field Experiment on Compound Interest in China. 2011 [ Google Scholar ]
- Stango Victor, Jonathan Zinman. Exponential Growth Bias and Household Finance. Journal of Finance. 2009;64:2807–2849. [ Google Scholar ]
- Tennyson Sharon, Nguyen Chau. State Curriculum Mandates and Student Knowledge of Personal Finance. Journal of Consumer Affairs. 2001;35(2):241–262. [ Google Scholar ]
- Thaler Richard, Benartzi Shlomo. Save More Tomorrow: Using Behavioral Economics to Increase Employee Saving. Journal of Political Economy. 2004;112(1, part 2):S164–S196. [ Google Scholar ]
- Thaler Richard, Sunstein Cass. Nudge: Improving Decisions About Health, Wealth, and Happiness. New Haven: Yale University Press; 2010. [ Google Scholar ]
- Thaler Richard, Sunstein Cass, Balz John. (SSRN Working Paper).Choice Architecture. 2010 http://ssrn.com/abstract=1583509 or http://dx.doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.1583509 .
- U.S. Securities and Exchange Commission (SEC) Study Regarding Financial Literacy Among Investors As Required by Section 917 of the Dodd-Frank Wall Street Reform and Consumer Protection Act. SEC Staff Study. 2012 Aug [ Google Scholar ]
- Utkus Stephen, Young Jean. Financial Literacy and 401(k) Loans. In: Mitchell OS, Lusardi A, editors. Financial Literacy: Implications for Retirement Security and the Financial Marketplace. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2011. pp. 59–75. [ Google Scholar ]
- Van Rooij Maarten, Lusardi Annamaria, Alessie Rob. Financial Literacy and Stock Market Participation. Journal of Financial Economics. 2011;101(2):449–472. [ Google Scholar ]
- Van Rooij Maarten, Lusardi Annamaria, Alessie Rob. Financial Literacy, Retirement Planning, and Households Wealth. Economic Journal. 2012;122:449–478. [ Google Scholar ]
- Venti Steven, Wise David. The Cause of Wealth Dispersion at Retirement: Choice or Chance? American Economic Review. 1998;88:185–191. [ Google Scholar ]
- Venti Steven, Wise David. Choice, Chance, and Wealth Dispersion. In: Ogura S, Tachibanaki T, Wise D, editors. Aging Issues in the United States and Japan. Chicago: University of Chicago Press; 2001. pp. 25–64. [ Google Scholar ]
- Walstad William, Rebeck Ken, MacDonald Richard. The Effects of Financial Education on the Financial Knowledge of High School Students. Journal of Consumer Affairs. 2010;44(2):336–357. [ Google Scholar ]
- Way Wendy, Holden Karen. Teachers’ Background and Capacity to Teach Personal Finance: Results of a National Study. Journal of Financial Counseling and Planning. 2009;20(2):64–78. [ Google Scholar ]
- Willis Lauren. Against Financial Literacy Education. University of Pennsylvania; 2008. (Public Law and Legal Theory Research Paper Series Research Paper No. #08-10). [ Google Scholar ]
- Willis Lauren. The Financial Education Fallacy. American Economic Review. 2011;101(3):429–434. [ Google Scholar ]
- Yoong Joanne. Financial Illiteracy and Stock Market Participation: Evidence from the RAND American Life Panel. In: Mitchell OS, Lusardi A, editors. Financial Literacy: Implications for Retirement Security and the Financial Marketplace. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2011. pp. 76–97. [ Google Scholar ]
- View on publisher site
- PDF (680.5 KB)
- Collections
Similar articles
Cited by other articles, links to ncbi databases.
- Download .nbib .nbib
- Format: AMA APA MLA NLM
Add to Collections
- Conference key note
- Open access
- Published: 24 January 2019
Financial literacy and the need for financial education: evidence and implications
- Annamaria Lusardi 1
Swiss Journal of Economics and Statistics volume 155 , Article number: 1 ( 2019 ) Cite this article
468k Accesses
366 Citations
217 Altmetric
Metrics details
1 Introduction
Throughout their lifetime, individuals today are more responsible for their personal finances than ever before. With life expectancies rising, pension and social welfare systems are being strained. In many countries, employer-sponsored defined benefit (DB) pension plans are swiftly giving way to private defined contribution (DC) plans, shifting the responsibility for retirement saving and investing from employers to employees. Individuals have also experienced changes in labor markets. Skills are becoming more critical, leading to divergence in wages between those with a college education, or higher, and those with lower levels of education. Simultaneously, financial markets are rapidly changing, with developments in technology and new and more complex financial products. From student loans to mortgages, credit cards, mutual funds, and annuities, the range of financial products people have to choose from is very different from what it was in the past, and decisions relating to these financial products have implications for individual well-being. Moreover, the exponential growth in financial technology (fintech) is revolutionizing the way people make payments, decide about their financial investments, and seek financial advice. In this context, it is important to understand how financially knowledgeable people are and to what extent their knowledge of finance affects their financial decision-making.
An essential indicator of people’s ability to make financial decisions is their level of financial literacy. The Organisation for Economic Co-operation and Development (OECD) aptly defines financial literacy as not only the knowledge and understanding of financial concepts and risks but also the skills, motivation, and confidence to apply such knowledge and understanding in order to make effective decisions across a range of financial contexts, to improve the financial well-being of individuals and society, and to enable participation in economic life. Thus, financial literacy refers to both knowledge and financial behavior, and this paper will analyze research on both topics.
As I describe in more detail below, findings around the world are sobering. Financial literacy is low even in advanced economies with well-developed financial markets. On average, about one third of the global population has familiarity with the basic concepts that underlie everyday financial decisions (Lusardi and Mitchell, 2011c ). The average hides gaping vulnerabilities of certain population subgroups and even lower knowledge of specific financial topics. Furthermore, there is evidence of a lack of confidence, particularly among women, and this has implications for how people approach and make financial decisions. In the following sections, I describe how we measure financial literacy, the levels of literacy we find around the world, the implications of those findings for financial decision-making, and how we can improve financial literacy.
2 How financially literate are people?
2.1 measuring financial literacy: the big three.
In the context of rapid changes and constant developments in the financial sector and the broader economy, it is important to understand whether people are equipped to effectively navigate the maze of financial decisions that they face every day. To provide the tools for better financial decision-making, one must assess not only what people know but also what they need to know, and then evaluate the gap between those things. There are a few fundamental concepts at the basis of most financial decision-making. These concepts are universal, applying to every context and economic environment. Three such concepts are (1) numeracy as it relates to the capacity to do interest rate calculations and understand interest compounding; (2) understanding of inflation; and (3) understanding of risk diversification. Translating these concepts into easily measured financial literacy metrics is difficult, but Lusardi and Mitchell ( 2008 , 2011b , 2011c ) have designed a standard set of questions around these concepts and implemented them in numerous surveys in the USA and around the world.
Four principles informed the design of these questions, as described in detail by Lusardi and Mitchell ( 2014 ). The first is simplicity : the questions should measure knowledge of the building blocks fundamental to decision-making in an intertemporal setting. The second is relevance : the questions should relate to concepts pertinent to peoples’ day-to-day financial decisions over the life cycle; moreover, they must capture general rather than context-specific ideas. Third is brevity : the number of questions must be few enough to secure widespread adoption; and fourth is capacity to differentiate , meaning that questions should differentiate financial knowledge in such a way as to permit comparisons across people. Each of these principles is important in the context of face-to-face, telephone, and online surveys.
Three basic questions (since dubbed the “Big Three”) to measure financial literacy have been fielded in many surveys in the USA, including the National Financial Capability Study (NFCS) and, more recently, the Survey of Consumer Finances (SCF), and in many national surveys around the world. They have also become the standard way to measure financial literacy in surveys used by the private sector. For example, the Aegon Center for Longevity and Retirement included the Big Three questions in the 2018 Aegon Retirement Readiness Survey, covering around 16,000 people in 15 countries. Both ING and Allianz, but also investment funds, and pension funds have used the Big Three to measure financial literacy. The exact wording of the questions is provided in Table 1 .
2.2 Cross-country comparison
The first examination of financial literacy using the Big Three was possible due to a special module on financial literacy and retirement planning that Lusardi and Mitchell designed for the 2004 Health and Retirement Study (HRS), which is a survey of Americans over age 50. Astonishingly, the data showed that only half of older Americans—who presumably had made many financial decisions in their lives—could answer the two basic questions measuring understanding of interest rates and inflation (Lusardi and Mitchell, 2011b ). And just one third demonstrated understanding of these two concepts and answered the third question, measuring understanding of risk diversification, correctly. It is sobering that recent US surveys, such as the 2015 NFCS, the 2016 SCF, and the 2017 Survey of Household Economics and Financial Decisionmaking (SHED), show that financial knowledge has remained stubbornly low over time.
Over time, the Big Three have been added to other national surveys across countries and Lusardi and Mitchell have coordinated a project called Financial Literacy around the World (FLat World), which is an international comparison of financial literacy (Lusardi and Mitchell, 2011c ).
Findings from the FLat World project, which so far includes data from 15 countries, including Switzerland, highlight the urgent need to improve financial literacy (see Table 2 ). Across countries, financial literacy is at a crisis level, with the average rate of financial literacy, as measured by those answering correctly all three questions, at around 30%. Moreover, only around 50% of respondents in most countries are able to correctly answer the two financial literacy questions on interest rates and inflation correctly. A noteworthy point is that most countries included in the FLat World project have well-developed financial markets, which further highlights the cause for alarm over the demonstrated lack of the financial literacy. The fact that levels of financial literacy are so similar across countries with varying levels of economic development—indicating that in terms of financial knowledge, the world is indeed flat —shows that income levels or ubiquity of complex financial products do not by themselves equate to a more financially literate population.
Other noteworthy findings emerge in Table 2 . For instance, as expected, understanding of the effects of inflation (i.e., of real versus nominal values) among survey respondents is low in countries that have experienced deflation rather than inflation: in Japan, understanding of inflation is at 59%; in other countries, such as Germany, it is at 78% and, in the Netherlands, it is at 77%. Across countries, individuals have the lowest level of knowledge around the concept of risk, and the percentage of correct answers is particularly low when looking at knowledge of risk diversification. Here, we note the prevalence of “do not know” answers. While “do not know” responses hover around 15% on the topic of interest rates and 18% for inflation, about 30% of respondents—in some countries even more—are likely to respond “do not know” to the risk diversification question. In Switzerland, 74% answered the risk diversification question correctly and 13% reported not knowing the answer (compared to 3% and 4% responding “do not know” for the interest rates and inflation questions, respectively).
These findings are supported by many other surveys. For example, the 2014 Standard & Poor’s Global Financial Literacy Survey shows that, around the world, people know the least about risk and risk diversification (Klapper, Lusardi, and Van Oudheusden, 2015 ). Similarly, results from the 2016 Allianz survey, which collected evidence from ten European countries on money, financial literacy, and risk in the digital age, show very low-risk literacy in all countries covered by the survey. In Austria, Germany, and Switzerland, which are the three top-performing nations in term of financial knowledge, less than 20% of respondents can answer three questions related to knowledge of risk and risk diversification (Allianz, 2017 ).
Other surveys show that the findings about financial literacy correlate in an expected way with other data. For example, performance on the mathematics and science sections of the OECD Program for International Student Assessment (PISA) correlates with performance on the Big Three and, specifically, on the question relating to interest rates. Similarly, respondents in Sweden, which has experienced pension privatization, performed better on the risk diversification question (at 68%), than did respondents in Russia and East Germany, where people have had less exposure to the stock market. For researchers studying financial knowledge and its effects, these findings hint to the fact that financial literacy could be the result of choice and not an exogenous variable.
To summarize, financial literacy is low across the world and higher national income levels do not equate to a more financially literate population. The design of the Big Three questions enables a global comparison and allows for a deeper understanding of financial literacy. This enhances the measure’s utility because it helps to identify general and specific vulnerabilities across countries and within population subgroups, as will be explained in the next section.
2.3 Who knows the least?
Low financial literacy on average is exacerbated by patterns of vulnerability among specific population subgroups. For instance, as reported in Lusardi and Mitchell ( 2014 ), even though educational attainment is positively correlated with financial literacy, it is not sufficient. Even well-educated people are not necessarily savvy about money. Financial literacy is also low among the young. In the USA, less than 30% of respondents can correctly answer the Big Three by age 40, even though many consequential financial decisions are made well before that age (see Fig. 1 ). Similarly, in Switzerland, only 45% of those aged 35 or younger are able to correctly answer the Big Three questions. Footnote 1 And if people may learn from making financial decisions, that learning seems limited. As shown in Fig. 1 , many older individuals, who have already made decisions, cannot answer three basic financial literacy questions.
Financial literacy across age in the USA. This figure shows the percentage of respondents who answered correctly all Big Three questions by age group (year 2015). Source: 2015 US National Financial Capability Study
A gender gap in financial literacy is also present across countries. Women are less likely than men to answer questions correctly. The gap is present not only on the overall scale but also within each topic, across countries of different income levels, and at different ages. Women are also disproportionately more likely to indicate that they do not know the answer to specific questions (Fig. 2 ), highlighting overconfidence among men and awareness of lack of knowledge among women. Even in Finland, which is a relatively equal society in terms of gender, 44% of men compared to 27% of women answer all three questions correctly and 18% of women give at least one “do not know” response versus less than 10% of men (Kalmi and Ruuskanen, 2017 ). These figures further reflect the universality of the Big Three questions. As reported in Fig. 2 , “do not know” responses among women are prevalent not only in European countries, for example, Switzerland, but also in North America (represented in the figure by the USA, though similar findings are reported in Canada) and in Asia (represented in the figure by Japan). Those interested in learning more about the differences in financial literacy across demographics and other characteristics can consult Lusardi and Mitchell ( 2011c , 2014 ).
Gender differences in the responses to the Big Three questions. Sources: USA—Lusardi and Mitchell, 2011c ; Japan—Sekita, 2011 ; Switzerland—Brown and Graf, 2013
3 Does financial literacy matter?
A growing number of financial instruments have gained importance, including alternative financial services such as payday loans, pawnshops, and rent to own stores that charge very high interest rates. Simultaneously, in the changing economic landscape, people are increasingly responsible for personal financial planning and for investing and spending their resources throughout their lifetime. We have witnessed changes not only in the asset side of household balance sheets but also in the liability side. For example, in the USA, many people arrive close to retirement carrying a lot more debt than previous generations did (Lusardi, Mitchell, and Oggero, 2018 ). Overall, individuals are making substantially more financial decisions over their lifetime, living longer, and gaining access to a range of new financial products. These trends, combined with low financial literacy levels around the world and, particularly, among vulnerable population groups, indicate that elevating financial literacy must become a priority for policy makers.
There is ample evidence of the impact of financial literacy on people’s decisions and financial behavior. For example, financial literacy has been proven to affect both saving and investment behavior and debt management and borrowing practices. Empirically, financially savvy people are more likely to accumulate wealth (Lusardi and Mitchell, 2014 ). There are several explanations for why higher financial literacy translates into greater wealth. Several studies have documented that those who have higher financial literacy are more likely to plan for retirement, probably because they are more likely to appreciate the power of interest compounding and are better able to do calculations. According to the findings of the FLat World project, answering one additional financial question correctly is associated with a 3–4 percentage point greater probability of planning for retirement; this finding is seen in Germany, the USA, Japan, and Sweden. Financial literacy is found to have the strongest impact in the Netherlands, where knowing the right answer to one additional financial literacy question is associated with a 10 percentage point higher probability of planning (Mitchell and Lusardi, 2015 ). Empirically, planning is a very strong predictor of wealth; those who plan arrive close to retirement with two to three times the amount of wealth as those who do not plan (Lusardi and Mitchell, 2011b ).
Financial literacy is also associated with higher returns on investments and investment in more complex assets, such as stocks, which normally offer higher rates of return. This finding has important consequences for wealth; according to the simulation by Lusardi, Michaud, and Mitchell ( 2017 ), in the context of a life-cycle model of saving with many sources of uncertainty, from 30 to 40% of US retirement wealth inequality can be accounted for by differences in financial knowledge. These results show that financial literacy is not a sideshow, but it plays a critical role in saving and wealth accumulation.
Financial literacy is also strongly correlated with a greater ability to cope with emergency expenses and weather income shocks. Those who are financially literate are more likely to report that they can come up with $2000 in 30 days or that they are able to cover an emergency expense of $400 with cash or savings (Hasler, Lusardi, and Oggero, 2018 ).
With regard to debt behavior, those who are more financially literate are less likely to have credit card debt and more likely to pay the full balance of their credit card each month rather than just paying the minimum due (Lusardi and Tufano, 2009 , 2015 ). Individuals with higher financial literacy levels also are more likely to refinance their mortgages when it makes sense to do so, tend not to borrow against their 401(k) plans, and are less likely to use high-cost borrowing methods, e.g., payday loans, pawn shops, auto title loans, and refund anticipation loans (Lusardi and de Bassa Scheresberg, 2013 ).
Several studies have documented poor debt behavior and its link to financial literacy. Moore ( 2003 ) reported that the least financially literate are also more likely to have costly mortgages. Lusardi and Tufano ( 2015 ) showed that the least financially savvy incurred high transaction costs, paying higher fees and using high-cost borrowing methods. In their study, the less knowledgeable also reported excessive debt loads and an inability to judge their debt positions. Similarly, Mottola ( 2013 ) found that those with low financial literacy were more likely to engage in costly credit card behavior, and Utkus and Young ( 2011 ) concluded that the least literate were more likely to borrow against their 401(k) and pension accounts.
Young people also struggle with debt, in particular with student loans. According to Lusardi, de Bassa Scheresberg, and Oggero ( 2016 ), Millennials know little about their student loans and many do not attempt to calculate the payment amounts that will later be associated with the loans they take. When asked what they would do, if given the chance to revisit their student loan borrowing decisions, about half of Millennials indicate that they would make a different decision.
Finally, a recent report on Millennials in the USA (18- to 34-year-olds) noted the impact of financial technology (fintech) on the financial behavior of young individuals. New and rapidly expanding mobile payment options have made transactions easier, quicker, and more convenient. The average user of mobile payments apps and technology in the USA is a high-income, well-educated male who works full time and is likely to belong to an ethnic minority group. Overall, users of mobile payments are busy individuals who are financially active (holding more assets and incurring more debt). However, mobile payment users display expensive financial behaviors, such as spending more than they earn, using alternative financial services, and occasionally overdrawing their checking accounts. Additionally, mobile payment users display lower levels of financial literacy (Lusardi, de Bassa Scheresberg, and Avery, 2018 ). The rapid growth in fintech around the world juxtaposed with expensive financial behavior means that more attention must be paid to the impact of mobile payment use on financial behavior. Fintech is not a substitute for financial literacy.
4 The way forward for financial literacy and what works
Overall, financial literacy affects everything from day-to-day to long-term financial decisions, and this has implications for both individuals and society. Low levels of financial literacy across countries are correlated with ineffective spending and financial planning, and expensive borrowing and debt management. These low levels of financial literacy worldwide and their widespread implications necessitate urgent efforts. Results from various surveys and research show that the Big Three questions are useful not only in assessing aggregate financial literacy but also in identifying vulnerable population subgroups and areas of financial decision-making that need improvement. Thus, these findings are relevant for policy makers and practitioners. Financial illiteracy has implications not only for the decisions that people make for themselves but also for society. The rapid spread of mobile payment technology and alternative financial services combined with lack of financial literacy can exacerbate wealth inequality.
To be effective, financial literacy initiatives need to be large and scalable. Schools, workplaces, and community platforms provide unique opportunities to deliver financial education to large and often diverse segments of the population. Furthermore, stark vulnerabilities across countries make it clear that specific subgroups, such as women and young people, are ideal targets for financial literacy programs. Given women’s awareness of their lack of financial knowledge, as indicated via their “do not know” responses to the Big Three questions, they are likely to be more receptive to financial education.
The near-crisis levels of financial illiteracy, the adverse impact that it has on financial behavior, and the vulnerabilities of certain groups speak of the need for and importance of financial education. Financial education is a crucial foundation for raising financial literacy and informing the next generations of consumers, workers, and citizens. Many countries have seen efforts in recent years to implement and provide financial education in schools, colleges, and workplaces. However, the continuously low levels of financial literacy across the world indicate that a piece of the puzzle is missing. A key lesson is that when it comes to providing financial education, one size does not fit all. In addition to the potential for large-scale implementation, the main components of any financial literacy program should be tailored content, targeted at specific audiences. An effective financial education program efficiently identifies the needs of its audience, accurately targets vulnerable groups, has clear objectives, and relies on rigorous evaluation metrics.
Using measures like the Big Three questions, it is imperative to recognize vulnerable groups and their specific needs in program designs. Upon identification, the next step is to incorporate this knowledge into financial education programs and solutions.
School-based education can be transformational by preparing young people for important financial decisions. The OECD’s Programme for International Student Assessment (PISA), in both 2012 and 2015, found that, on average, only 10% of 15-year-olds achieved maximum proficiency on a five-point financial literacy scale. As of 2015, about one in five of students did not have even basic financial skills (see OECD, 2017 ). Rigorous financial education programs, coupled with teacher training and high school financial education requirements, are found to be correlated with fewer defaults and higher credit scores among young adults in the USA (Urban, Schmeiser, Collins, and Brown, 2018 ). It is important to target students and young adults in schools and colleges to provide them with the necessary tools to make sound financial decisions as they graduate and take on responsibilities, such as buying cars and houses, or starting retirement accounts. Given the rising cost of education and student loan debt and the need of young people to start contributing as early as possible to retirement accounts, the importance of financial education in school cannot be overstated.
There are three compelling reasons for having financial education in school. First, it is important to expose young people to the basic concepts underlying financial decision-making before they make important and consequential financial decisions. As noted in Fig. 1 , financial literacy is very low among the young and it does not seem to increase a lot with age/generations. Second, school provides access to financial literacy to groups who may not be exposed to it (or may not be equally exposed to it), for example, women. Third, it is important to reduce the costs of acquiring financial literacy, if we want to promote higher financial literacy both among individuals and among society.
There are compelling reasons to have personal finance courses in college as well. In the same way in which colleges and university offer courses in corporate finance to teach how to manage the finances of firms, so today individuals need the knowledge to manage their own finances over the lifetime, which in present discounted value often amount to large values and are made larger by private pension accounts.
Financial education can also be efficiently provided in workplaces. An effective financial education program targeted to adults recognizes the socioeconomic context of employees and offers interventions tailored to their specific needs. A case study conducted in 2013 with employees of the US Federal Reserve System showed that completing a financial literacy learning module led to significant changes in retirement planning behavior and better-performing investment portfolios (Clark, Lusardi, and Mitchell, 2017 ). It is also important to note the delivery method of these programs, especially when targeted to adults. For instance, video formats have a significantly higher impact on financial behavior than simple narratives, and instruction is most effective when it is kept brief and relevant (Heinberg et al., 2014 ).
The Big Three also show that it is particularly important to make people familiar with the concepts of risk and risk diversification. Programs devoted to teaching risk via, for example, visual tools have shown great promise (Lusardi et al., 2017 ). The complexity of some of these concepts and the costs of providing education in the workplace, coupled with the fact that many older individuals may not work or work in firms that do not offer such education, provide other reasons why financial education in school is so important.
Finally, it is important to provide financial education in the community, in places where people go to learn. A recent example is the International Federation of Finance Museums, an innovative global collaboration that promotes financial knowledge through museum exhibits and the exchange of resources. Museums can be places where to provide financial literacy both among the young and the old.
There are a variety of other ways in which financial education can be offered and also targeted to specific groups. However, there are few evaluations of the effectiveness of such initiatives and this is an area where more research is urgently needed, given the statistics reported in the first part of this paper.
5 Concluding remarks
The lack of financial literacy, even in some of the world’s most well-developed financial markets, is of acute concern and needs immediate attention. The Big Three questions that were designed to measure financial literacy go a long way in identifying aggregate differences in financial knowledge and highlighting vulnerabilities within populations and across topics of interest, thereby facilitating the development of tailored programs. Many such programs to provide financial education in schools and colleges, workplaces, and the larger community have taken existing evidence into account to create rigorous solutions. It is important to continue making strides in promoting financial literacy, by achieving scale and efficiency in future programs as well.
In August 2017, I was appointed Director of the Italian Financial Education Committee, tasked with designing and implementing the national strategy for financial literacy. I will be able to apply my research to policy and program initiatives in Italy to promote financial literacy: it is an essential skill in the twenty-first century, one that individuals need if they are to thrive economically in today’s society. As the research discussed in this paper well documents, financial literacy is like a global passport that allows individuals to make the most of the plethora of financial products available in the market and to make sound financial decisions. Financial literacy should be seen as a fundamental right and universal need, rather than the privilege of the relatively few consumers who have special access to financial knowledge or financial advice. In today’s world, financial literacy should be considered as important as basic literacy, i.e., the ability to read and write. Without it, individuals and societies cannot reach their full potential.
See Brown and Graf ( 2013 ).
Abbreviations
Defined benefit (refers to pension plan)
Defined contribution (refers to pension plan)
Financial Literacy around the World
National Financial Capability Study
Organisation for Economic Co-operation and Development
Programme for International Student Assessment
Survey of Consumer Finances
Survey of Household Economics and Financial Decisionmaking
Aegon Center for Longevity and Retirement. (2018). The New Social Contract: a blueprint for retirement in the 21st century. The Aegon Retirement Readiness Survey 2018. Retrieved from https://www.aegon.com/en/Home/Research/aegon-retirement-readiness-survey-2018/ . Accessed 1 June 2018.
Agnew, J., Bateman, H., & Thorp, S. (2013). Financial literacy and retirement planning in Australia. Numeracy, 6 (2).
Allianz (2017). When will the penny drop? Money, financial literacy and risk in the digital age. Retrieved from http://gflec.org/initiatives/money-finlit-risk/ . Accessed 1 June 2018.
Almenberg, J., & Säve-Söderbergh, J. (2011). Financial literacy and retirement planning in Sweden. Journal of Pension Economics & Finance, 10 (4), 585–598.
Article Google Scholar
Arrondel, L., Debbich, M., & Savignac, F. (2013). Financial literacy and financial planning in France. Numeracy, 6 (2).
Beckmann, E. (2013). Financial literacy and household savings in Romania. Numeracy, 6 (2).
Boisclair, D., Lusardi, A., & Michaud, P. C. (2017). Financial literacy and retirement planning in Canada. Journal of Pension Economics & Finance, 16 (3), 277–296.
Brown, M., & Graf, R. (2013). Financial literacy and retirement planning in Switzerland. Numeracy, 6 (2).
Bucher-Koenen, T., & Lusardi, A. (2011). Financial literacy and retirement planning in Germany. Journal of Pension Economics & Finance, 10 (4), 565–584.
Clark, R., Lusardi, A., & Mitchell, O. S. (2017). Employee financial literacy and retirement plan behavior: a case study. Economic Inquiry, 55 (1), 248–259.
Crossan, D., Feslier, D., & Hurnard, R. (2011). Financial literacy and retirement planning in New Zealand. Journal of Pension Economics & Finance, 10 (4), 619–635.
Fornero, E., & Monticone, C. (2011). Financial literacy and pension plan participation in Italy. Journal of Pension Economics & Finance, 10 (4), 547–564.
Hasler, A., Lusardi, A., and Oggero, N. (2018). Financial fragility in the US: evidence and implications. GFLEC working paper n. 2018–1.
Heinberg, A., Hung, A., Kapteyn, A., Lusardi, A., Samek, A. S., & Yoong, J. (2014). Five steps to planning success: experimental evidence from US households. Oxford Review of Economic Policy, 30 (4), 697–724.
Kalmi, P., & Ruuskanen, O. P. (2017). Financial literacy and retirement planning in Finland. Journal of Pension Economics & Finance, 17 (3), 1–28.
Klapper, L., Lusardi, A., & Van Oudheusden, P. (2015). Financial literacy around the world. In Standard & Poor’s Ratings Services Global Financial Literacy Survey (GFLEC working paper).
Google Scholar
Klapper, L., & Panos, G. A. (2011). Financial literacy and retirement planning: The Russian case. Journal of Pension Economics & Finance, 10 (4), 599–618.
Lusardi, A., & de Bassa Scheresberg, C. (2013). Financial literacy and high-cost borrowing in the United States, NBER Working Paper n. 18969, April .
Book Google Scholar
Lusardi, A., de Bassa Scheresberg, C., and Avery, M. 2018. Millennial mobile payment users: a look into their personal finances and financial behaviors. GFLEC working paper.
Lusardi, A., de Bassa Scheresberg, C., & Oggero, N. (2016). Student loan debt in the US: an analysis of the 2015 NFCS Data, GFLEC Policy Brief, November .
Lusardi, A., Michaud, P. C., & Mitchell, O. S. (2017). Optimal financial knowledge and wealth inequality. Journal of Political Economy, 125 (2), 431–477.
Lusardi, A., & Mitchell, O. S. (2008). Planning and financial literacy: how do women fare? American Economic Review, 98 , 413–417.
Lusardi, A., & Mitchell, O. S. (2011a). The outlook for financial literacy. In O. S. Mitchell & A. Lusardi (Eds.), Financial literacy: implications for retirement security and the financial marketplace (pp. 1–15). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Chapter Google Scholar
Lusardi, A., & Mitchell, O. S. (2011b). Financial literacy and planning: implications for retirement wellbeing. In O. S. Mitchell & A. Lusardi (Eds.), Financial literacy: implications for retirement security and the financial marketplace (pp. 17–39). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Lusardi, A., & Mitchell, O. S. (2011c). Financial literacy around the world: an overview. Journal of Pension Economics and Finance, 10 (4), 497–508.
Lusardi, A., & Mitchell, O. S. (2014). The economic importance of financial literacy: theory and evidence. Journal of Economic Literature, 52 (1), 5–44.
Lusardi, A., Mitchell, O. S., & Oggero, N. (2018). The changing face of debt and financial fragility at older ages. American Economic Association Papers and Proceedings, 108 , 407–411.
Lusardi, A., Samek, A., Kapteyn, A., Glinert, L., Hung, A., & Heinberg, A. (2017). Visual tools and narratives: new ways to improve financial literacy. Journal of Pension Economics & Finance, 16 (3), 297–323.
Lusardi, A., & Tufano, P. (2009). Teach workers about the peril of debt. Harvard Business Review , 22–24.
Lusardi, A., & Tufano, P. (2015). Debt literacy, financial experiences, and overindebtedness. Journal of Pension Economics & Finance, 14 (4), 332–368.
Mitchell, O. S., & Lusardi, A. (2015). Financial literacy and economic outcomes: evidence and policy implications. The Journal of Retirement, 3 (1).
Moore, Danna. 2003. Survey of financial literacy in Washington State: knowledge, behavior, attitudes and experiences. Washington State University Social and Economic Sciences Research Center Technical Report 03–39.
Mottola, G. R. (2013). In our best interest: women, financial literacy, and credit card behavior. Numeracy, 6 (2).
Moure, N. G. (2016). Financial literacy and retirement planning in Chile. Journal of Pension Economics & Finance, 15 (2), 203–223.
OECD. (2017). PISA 2015 results (Volume IV): students’ financial literacy . Paris: PISA, OECD Publishing. https://doi.org/10.1787/9789264270282-en .
Sekita, S. (2011). Financial literacy and retirement planning in Japan. Journal of Pension Economics & Finance, 10 (4), 637–656.
Urban, C., Schmeiser, M., Collins, J. M., & Brown, A. (2018). The effects of high school personal financial education policies on financial behavior. Economics of Education Review . https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/abs/pii/S0272775718301699 .
Utkus, S., & Young, J. (2011). Financial literacy and 401(k) loans. In O. S. Mitchell & A. Lusardi (Eds.), Financial literacy: implications for retirement security and the financial marketplace (pp. 59–75). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Van Rooij, M. C., Lusardi, A., & Alessie, R. J. (2011). Financial literacy and retirement preparation in the Netherlands. Journal of Pension Economics and Finance, 10 (4), 527–545.
Download references
Acknowledgements
This paper represents a summary of the keynote address I gave to the 2018 Annual Meeting of the Swiss Society of Economics and Statistics. I would like to thank Monika Butler, Rafael Lalive, anonymous reviewers, and participants of the Annual Meeting for useful discussions and comments, and Raveesha Gupta for editorial support. All errors are my responsibility.
Not applicable
Availability of data and materials
Author information, authors and affiliations.
The George Washington University School of Business Global Financial Literacy Excellence Center and Italian Committee for Financial Education, Washington, D.C., USA
Annamaria Lusardi
You can also search for this author in PubMed Google Scholar
Contributions
The author read and approved the final manuscript.
Corresponding author
Correspondence to Annamaria Lusardi .
Ethics declarations
Competing interests.
The author declares that she has no competing interests.
Publisher’s Note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License ( http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ ), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made.
Reprints and permissions
About this article
Cite this article.
Lusardi, A. Financial literacy and the need for financial education: evidence and implications. Swiss J Economics Statistics 155 , 1 (2019). https://doi.org/10.1186/s41937-019-0027-5
Download citation
Received : 22 October 2018
Accepted : 07 January 2019
Published : 24 January 2019
DOI : https://doi.org/10.1186/s41937-019-0027-5
Share this article
Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content:
Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article.
Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative
REVIEW article
Youth, money, and behavior: the impact of financial literacy programs.
- Department of Human Sciences, Society and Health, University of Cassino and Southern Lazio, Cassino, Lazio, Italy
This narrative review critically examines the scope and impact of financial literacy programs targeted at children and adolescents. By synthesizing findings from diverse studies, the review identifies key components of effective financial education, including the integration of experiential learning, the role of digital tools, and the importance of parental involvement. Challenges such as ensuring curriculum relevance in a rapidly evolving financial landscape and addressing the varied learning needs of young populations are discussed. Recommendations for future research include the necessity for longitudinal studies to assess the long-term effects of early financial education and exploration into the digitalization of financial literacy. This review aims to contribute to the development of more inclusive, adaptive, and impactful financial literacy education strategies, underscoring the critical role of comprehensive financial education in preparing young individuals for financial autonomy and resilience.
1 Introduction
Financial literacy among young people today is increasingly important as finances become more complex. Today's youth, specifically those aged 6–18 years, are growing up in the most financially turbulent and challenging world that we have ever seen. This study focuses on students from primary to secondary school, examining the impact of financial education interventions on children and adolescents within this age group. The evolving financial landscape necessitates the early introduction of financial education to this demographic to help them develop the knowledge and skills needed to make informed financial decisions.
Caplinska and Ohotina (2019) highlight that a well-designed financial education curriculum can equip individuals to navigate the complexities of the economy and finance, fostering productivity and adaptability. Moscarola and Kalwij (2021) further stress the significance of early financial literacy education for children, underlining why and how financial literacy should be instilled from a young age. By introducing financial literacy at a young age, individuals can develop the knowledge and skills needed to make informed financial decisions, navigate the intricate financial environment, and contribute to their financial wellbeing and that of society as a whole.
The literature emphasizes the critical importance of early financial education for both individual financial wellbeing and broader societal implications. Letkiewicz and Fox (2014) highlight the role of financial literacy in asset accumulation and informed financial decision-making. Lührmann et al. (2015) stress the practical significance of enhancing financial literacy among teenagers, who generally exhibit lower financial literacy levels compared to adults. These studies underscore the necessity of equipping young individuals with the essential financial knowledge and skills to make informed decisions and navigate the complexities of the financial landscape.
Early financial education plays a pivotal role in shaping individuals' financial behaviors and attitudes. Garg and Singh (2018) discuss the positive relationship between financial education and financial literacy among youth, emphasizing the importance of financial education programs in enhancing financial knowledge. Also Chhatwani and Mishra (2021) suggest that financial literacy is a critical tool to enhance financial wellbeing, indicating the need for early financial education to empower individuals to make sound financial decisions.
Literature suggests that financial literacy is crucial for individual financial wellbeing and broader societal implications. Studies by Eniola and Entebang (2017) and Hussain et al. (2018) highlight the positive impact of early financial education on economic understanding and financial behavior, contributing to overall financial stability. The research by Letkiewicz et al. (2019) and Zhou et al. (2023) further emphasizes the importance of financial literacy in promoting asset accumulation and informed financial decision-making, underscoring the need for comprehensive financial education from a young age. To effectively enhance adolescents' and children's understanding of money management, it is essential to provide them with comprehensive financial education programs from an early age. Research by Marchetti et al. (2021) underscores the importance of introducing financial concepts and skills to children in a structured and engaging manner to establish a solid foundation for their financial literacy development.
Fuente-Mella et al. (2021) highlight that the family serves as the primary source of financial literacy for children, emphasizing the crucial role of parental involvement in teaching children about money management and financial decision-making. Parents can act as role models and mentors, demonstrating responsible financial behaviors that significantly influence children's financial literacy development. Gardynia and Syaodih (2021) discuss the significance of integrating financial literacy education into early childhood learning using cognitive learning theories by renowned psychologists. This approach aids children in grasping fundamental financial concepts and acquiring essential money management skills from a young age, facilitating effective application in real-life scenarios. This early exposure to financial education shapes children's attitudes and behaviors toward money management, laying a robust foundation for their financial wellbeing in adulthood.
Young people can be taught to monitor their spending so that it does not exceed their budget and accounts can be used to this end. All of this can aid them in making conscious decisions that are not based on impulse or influenced by what their friends are doing. The use of technology can help children to engage with the process of deciding how to spend money. For example, comparing prices and features of items that they might wish to buy over the internet. Pahlevan Sharif and Naghavi (2020) stress the importance of financial literacy among today's adolescents and youth, as they are increasingly exposed to financial decision-making. This underscores the necessity of equipping young individuals with the knowledge and skills required to monitor their spending effectively and make informed financial choices.
Despite growing recognition of its importance, financial literacy among children and adolescents remains a significantly underexplored area. Existing research has predominantly focused on adults, leaving a critical gap in understanding how early financial education influences long-term financial behaviors and decision-making. Addressing this gap is essential, as early interventions can shape financial attitudes and capabilities, potentially leading to more financially responsible adults ( Mallia et al., 2020 ; Diotaiuti et al., 2022a , b ; Mancone et al., 2024a , b ). This research gap underscores the need for comprehensive studies that evaluate the effectiveness of financial literacy programs targeted at younger populations, exploring variables such as age, socio-economic background, and educational content delivery methods.
This study aims to review the existing literature on financial literacy education among children and adolescents. The following research questions were developed to guide the review: (1) What are the key components of effective financial literacy education for children and adolescents? This question seeks to identify the essential elements that contribute to successful financial education programs, including content, teaching methods, and delivery platforms. (2) How do financial literacy education needs differ between primary school children (ages 6–12) and teenagers (ages 13–18)? This issue would explore the developmental differences in financial education needs, addressing how cognitive and behavioral changes impact the design and effectiveness of educational interventions. (3) What evidence exists on the effectiveness of different types of financial literacy interventions (e.g., school-based programs, family workshops, and digital platforms)? This question would focus on evaluating the empirical evidence regarding various intervention types, assessing their impact on financial knowledge, attitudes, and behaviors. (4) What are the theoretical frameworks that underpin financial literacy education for young people? This question aims to identify the theoretical models that inform financial literacy education, including developmental psychology, social learning theories, and behavioral economics. (5) What gaps exist in the current research on financial literacy education for children and adolescents, and what are the implications for future studies? This question highlights areas where further research is needed, providing recommendations for future studies to address current limitations and emerging needs.
2 Methodology
This review systematically examines financial literacy among children and adolescents, focusing on individuals aged 6–18 years. Our geographical scope is global, encompassing studies from diverse economic backgrounds to understand universal and region-specific educational outcomes. The types of financial education interventions reviewed include school-based programs, family and community workshops, and digital learning platforms.
We followed a systematic approach in line with the PRISMA (Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic reviews and Meta-Analyses) framework to ensure a transparent and rigorous selection process. A comprehensive search of academic databases, including PubMed, PsycINFO, ERIC, and Google Scholar, was conducted. The literature considered comprises peer-reviewed articles, educational reports, policy analyses, and seminal books published in the field of financial education.
Our search strategy employed a combination of keywords and phrases to capture the breadth of the topic. The exact search string used was: (“financial literacy” OR “financial education”) AND (“children” OR “adolescents” OR “youth”) AND (“financial behavior” OR “financial intervention”) AND (“early financial education” OR “school-based financial programs” OR “family financial workshops” OR “digital learning platforms”). The selection process involved several stages: (1) we identified relevant studies through database searches, employing the specified keywords to cover the broad scope of financial literacy in children and adolescents; (2) titles and abstracts were screened to remove irrelevant studies, duplicates, and articles that did not meet the inclusion criteria; (3) full texts of the remaining articles were assessed for eligibility, focusing on empirical research, comprehensive reviews, and studies with significant sample sizes; (4) final studies were selected based on their contribution to understanding the effects of financial education on young people, with preference given to those published in the last two decades to ensure relevance and contemporary insights. Exclusion criteria eliminated non-English publications, studies focusing solely on adults, and research with inconclusive or non-replicable results. Table 1 presents a detailed summary of the criteria applied.
Table 1 . Criteria applied in the selection process.
This review examines a total of 80 unique studies on financial literacy, spanning from foundational theories to practical interventions and their effectiveness. The studies cover a broad temporal range, with publications spanning from early foundational works to the most recent research conducted up to 2023. The distribution of the studies is structured into five key sections, reflecting the diversity of approaches and findings in financial literacy research: (1) Theoretical Frameworks: outlines the foundational theories that guide the design of financial literacy education, providing context for the methodologies used. (2) Age-Specific Educational Needs: addresses how financial education requirements differ between primary school children and teenagers due to their cognitive and developmental stages. (3) Types of Financial Literacy Interventions: examines the variety of educational approaches, including school-based programs, family involvement, and digital platforms, highlighting their unique contributions. (4) Effectiveness of Financial Literacy Programs: focuses on studies that evaluate the impact of specific financial literacy interventions, offering evidence-based insights into what works. (5) Factors Influencing Financial Literacy: explores the various demographic, socioeconomic, psychological, and cultural factors that impact financial literacy levels among children, adolescents, and young adults. These categories were chosen to reflect the distinct areas that influence financial literacy education for children and adolescents, allowing for a comprehensive analysis of the literature.
3.1 Theoretical frameworks
Developmental psychology offers insights into how financial capabilities and the understanding of value evolve with cognitive development. This perspective suggests tailored financial education that grows with the child, introducing more complex concepts as their cognitive abilities mature. The understanding and interaction with financial concepts among children and adolescents are framed within several theories. Piaget (1952) 's Cognitive Development Theory suggests that children's ability to understand complex concepts like finance evolves through specific stages, with abstract thinking and operational skills developing in adolescence. Vygotsky (1978) 's Sociocultural Theory emphasizes the role of social interactions in learning, suggesting that children learn financial concepts through guided participation with adults and peers. Bandura (1977) 's Social Learning Theory highlights the importance of observation, imitation, and modeling, suggesting that children and adolescents learn financial behaviors by observing and mimicking the financial actions of others, especially parents and caregivers. These theories collectively underline the importance of age-appropriate financial education and the influence of social environments on financial literacy development.
Beyond the foundational theories, the Theory of Financial Socialization posits that children and adolescents develop financial attitudes and behaviors through the process of socialization, influenced by family, peers, and societal norms ( LeBaron and Kelley, 2021 ). This theory underscores the importance of early financial education and experiences, suggesting that positive early interactions with money can lead to better financial decisions in adulthood. Additionally, the Behavioral Economics Perspective introduces the idea that cognitive biases and psychological factors influence financial decision-making, highlighting the need for financial education programs to address these biases ( Zaleskiewicz and Traczyk, 2020 ; Zhu and Chou, 2020 ). Integrating these theories into educational practices can foster a more nuanced understanding of financial literacy among young individuals, preparing them for the financial realities of adult life.
Finally, the Ecological Systems Theory, proposed by Bronfenbrenner, can be applied to understand how various environmental systems, from immediate family and schools to broader societal influences, affect the financial learning and behavior of children and adolescents ( LeBaron-Black et al., 2022 ; Ullah and Yusheng, 2020 ). This theory emphasizes the interconnectedness of a child's development with their environment, suggesting that a supportive ecosystem can enhance financial literacy. Integrating community resources, policy initiatives, and technology within financial education can create a holistic learning environment that supports the diverse needs of young individuals, fostering a generation that is not only financially literate but also capable of making informed financial decisions in a complex world.
Table 2 presents the key theoretical frameworks that underpin financial literacy education, detailing the studies, participant ages, main topics investigated, and key findings. This table summarizes how different theories, such as Piaget's Cognitive Development Theory, Vygotsky's Sociocultural Theory, and Bandura's Social Learning Theory, contribute to our understanding of financial literacy development.
Table 2 . Theoretical frameworks in financial literacy education.
3.2 Age-specific educational needs
3.2.1 financial literacy in children.
Measuring and evaluating financial literacy in children involves various methods and tools designed to capture their understanding of financial concepts, decision-making skills, and attitudes toward money ( Sari et al., 2022 ; Fauziaha and Sari, 2019 ; Amagir et al., 2018 ). Standardized questionnaires and tests are commonly used to assess basic financial knowledge, including concepts of saving, spending, investing, and money use, and can range from multiple-choice questions to open-ended responses requiring detailed answers. Simulations and educational games enable children to engage with simulated financial scenarios, assessing how they apply their knowledge in practical situations such as budgeting, investment decisions, and spending choices. Interviews and behavioral observations, conducted by educators or researchers, provide insights into children's financial understanding, motivations, and attitudes toward money through direct interaction or observation of practical activities ( Sari et al., 2022 ). Financial diaries, where children record their financial decisions like saving pocket money, offer a longitudinal view of their financial behavior and planning ability. Project-based assessments, such as managing a small budget for a school activity or creating a savings plan, help evaluate how children apply financial knowledge in real-world contexts ( Merry et al., 2022 ). Lastly, feedback from parents and teachers offers valuable insights into children's financial behavior and their reactions to specific financial education programs, adding depth to the evaluation process ( Fauziaha and Sari, 2019 ; Amagir et al., 2018 ). It's also crucial to incorporate feedback mechanisms within these assessment methods to foster a culture of continuous improvement in financial literacy education. Engaging children in reflective practices on their financial decisions and the outcomes of educational games or simulations can deepen their understanding and self-awareness regarding financial matters ( Saputra and Susanti, 2021 ).
The role of digital literacy in financial education cannot be overstated. With the increasing prevalence of digital financial platforms, incorporating elements of digital financial literacy, such as understanding online transactions, digital wallets, and the basics of online financial security, into assessments can ensure that children are prepared for the digital aspects of financial management. In aligning assessment strategies with contemporary educational standards, it's beneficial to leverage technology not just as a medium for delivering financial education but also for assessing it. Online platforms and apps designed for financial education can offer personalized learning experiences and detailed tracking of progress, enabling educators to tailor interventions to individual needs ( Yin et al., 2022 ).
Ultimately, the goal of assessing financial literacy in children is not only to identify areas for improvement but also to celebrate progress and instill confidence in financial decision-making from a young age. By adopting a comprehensive and adaptive approach to assessment, educators and parents can work together to build a foundation of financial literacy that supports lifelong learning and financial wellbeing ( Barrot et al., 2024 ; Permana et al., 2021 ).
Building on the holistic approach to financial literacy assessment in children, it's essential to emphasize the importance of adaptability and inclusivity in these educational endeavors. Tailoring financial literacy programs and their assessments to accommodate diverse learning styles, cultural backgrounds, and socioeconomic statuses ensures that all children have the opportunity to develop a robust understanding of financial concepts. This inclusivity fosters an educational environment where financial literacy is seen not just as a set of skills to be acquired, but as a critical component of overall personal development and empowerment ( Indratirta et al., 2023 ).
Engaging stakeholders (parents, educators, financial experts, and the children themselves) in the development and refinement of financial literacy assessments can enhance the relevance and effectiveness of these tools. Stakeholder feedback can identify gaps in the curriculum, uncover innovative teaching methods, and ensure that the assessments are comprehensive and aligned with real-world financial challenges ( Kalwij et al., 2019 ).
In order to support continuous learning and adaptation, the integration of financial literacy education into broader life skills curricula is beneficial. This approach can facilitate the connection between financial decisions and other aspects of life, such as health, career choices, and civic engagement, thereby enriching the educational experience and preparing children for the complexities of adult life.
Assessment of financial literacy in children is a dynamic and multifaceted process that requires ongoing evaluation and adaptation. By embracing a comprehensive, inclusive, and stakeholder-engaged approach, educators and parents can significantly contribute to the development of financially literate, confident, and capable young individuals ready to navigate the financial aspects of their lives successfully ( Perdanasari et al., 2019 ).
Table 3 provides an overview of studies focused on assessing financial literacy in children, highlighting the different approaches used, participant age ranges, main topics investigated, and key findings. This table illustrates how these methods contribute to a holistic evaluation of financial literacy among young learners, offering insights into best practices for engaging and educating children in financial concepts.
Table 3 . Financial literacy in children.
3.2.2 Financial literacy in adolescents
As adolescents face increasingly complex financial decisions, including those related to retirement savings and investments, the need for enhanced financial literacy becomes more pronounced ( Sutter et al., 2019 ). Studies have also highlighted the positive association between financial literacy and various financial behaviors such as savings, investments, and wealth accumulation, indicating that financially literate individuals are better prepared for their financial future ( Kadoya and Khan, 2020 ). Assessing financial literacy in adolescents involves evaluating their understanding and application of financial concepts, skills, and behaviors necessary for making informed financial decisions, a crucial process for preparing young individuals to navigate the complexities of financial management in adulthood ( Yin et al., 2022 ; Liu et al., 2019 ; Lührmann et al., 2018 ). Key methods used in this evaluation include knowledge assessments, which typically involve quizzes or tests measuring adolescents' grasp of financial concepts such as saving, budgeting, investing, and borrowing, ranging from simple definitions to complex application scenarios. Behavioral assessments focus on observing real-life financial behaviors, such as saving habits, spending patterns, and the use of financial products, with surveys or interviews gathering data on these behaviors. Attitudinal assessments explore adolescents' attitudes toward money, assessing their confidence in financial decision-making, risk tolerance, and future financial expectations.
Simulations and role-playing scenarios are also employed, allowing adolescents to engage in practical exercises like budgeting, investment decision-making, or negotiating financial contracts, providing a hands-on approach to evaluating financial knowledge application. As digital finance becomes increasingly important, assessing digital financial literacy, including skills in navigating online financial services, understanding digital payment systems, and recognizing online scams, is essential. Curriculum-based assessments are another key component, integrating evaluations into formal education through project-based assessments, portfolio evaluations, and reflective essays on personal financial planning.
Effective assessment of financial literacy in adolescents combines these varied methods to capture a comprehensive view of their financial knowledge, behaviors, and attitudes. These assessments must also be tailored to be age-appropriate and culturally relevant, ensuring they remain accessible and engaging for young learners ( Yin et al., 2022 ; Liu et al., 2019 ; Lührmann et al., 2018 ). Integrating feedback mechanisms and continuous improvement processes into the evaluation framework is essential not only to assess current levels of financial literacy but also to foster a growth mindset among students, encouraging ongoing learning and development ( Kaiser and Menkhoff, 2020 ; Lučić et al., 2020 ; Amagir et al., 2019 ; Cordero and Pedraja, 2019 ; Arofah et al., 2018 ; Wolla, 2017 ). A comprehensive approach to financial literacy assessment should include various considerations, such as peer assessments and collaborative learning, which motivate adolescents to engage with financial concepts together, promoting teamwork and enhancing their social learning experience. Longitudinal studies provide valuable insights into the development of financial literacy over time, allowing educators to track changes in knowledge, behaviors, and attitudes, and to identify the impacts of financial education interventions.
Incorporating technology into assessments makes evaluations more interactive and accessible, engaging adolescents through online quizzes, financial literacy apps, and virtual simulations that offer immediate feedback and personalized learning experiences. Aligning assessments with real-world financial challenges ensures relevance by reflecting situations adolescents may encounter, such as case studies and discussions of contemporary financial issues. Feedback from adolescents on the assessment process and the financial literacy program itself offers critical insights into effectiveness and areas for improvement, helping tailor future curricula to better meet their needs.
Integrating career and life planning contextualizes the importance of financial skills in achieving personal success, contributing to the holistic development of financially savvy individuals. Cross-disciplinary approaches highlight the interconnected nature of financial literacy with subjects like mathematics, social studies, and technology, helping students apply their broader education practically. Engaging communities and families in the educational process, through workshops and projects, provides real-world contexts for financial decision-making, strengthening learning outcomes.
Adapting assessments to changing financial landscapes ensures that they remain up-to-date with current financial trends, preparing students for future challenges. Promoting ethical and responsible financial behavior evaluates students' understanding of consumer rights, ethical investing, and the social impacts of financial decisions. Incorporating global perspectives helps students appreciate the interconnectedness of the world economy and the importance of understanding diverse financial systems. By adopting these strategies, educators can create a dynamic and responsive financial literacy curriculum that equips adolescents for the complexities of modern financial environments. This approach promotes financial competence, critical thinking, ethical decision-making, and a sense of global citizenship, preparing young people to navigate real financial challenges effectively ( Silinskas et al., 2023 ; Kaiser et al., 2022 ; Frisancho, 2020 ; Klapper and Lusardi, 2020 ).
Table 4 provides an overview of key studies focused on financial literacy in adolescents, detailing the main topics investigated, participants' age ranges, and significant findings from each study. The table includes a variety of assessment methods, such as knowledge and behavioral assessments, digital literacy evaluations, and feedback mechanisms that foster continuous learning and growth.
Table 4 . Financial literacy in adolescents.
3.3 Types of financial literacy interventions
Education programs utilize various approaches, including school-based curricula, family involvement, and digital tools. Research has shown that school-based financial education programs effectively improve financial knowledge and behaviors among students. These programs typically cover topics such as spending, saving, budgeting, and investment ( Popovich et al., 2020 ; Kalwij et al., 2019 ; Amagir et al., 2019 ; Batty et al., 2015 ). Involving parents in financial education initiatives has been found to increase the financial literacy of children and adolescents ( Amagir et al., 2018 ).
Digital tools are increasingly integrated into financial education programs to engage students and enhance learning outcomes. These tools offer interactive platforms for students to learn about financial concepts in an engaging manner. Digital tools make financial education more accessible and convenient for students, allowing them to learn at their own pace ( Jonker and Kosse, 2022 ; Johnson et al., 2021 ).
The effectiveness of financial education programs is influenced by the quality of teacher training and professional development in delivering financial education curricula. Teachers play a crucial role in imparting financial knowledge and skills to students, and their training is vital for the successful implementation of financial education programs ( Compen et al., 2019 ; Collins and Odders-White, 2015 ). Incorporating innovative approaches and emerging trends significantly enhances the effectiveness and reach of financial literacy education. Programs that integrate financial education with Social and Emotional Learning (SEL) principles focus on developing comprehensive life skills including decision-making, goal-setting, empathy, and self-awareness alongside financial literacy. This holistic approach, recognizing the emotional aspects of financial decision-making, aims to build a more rounded set of life skills ( Mahoney et al., 2021 ).
The use of gamification and competitive learning introduces elements of competition and reward to make learning about finances more engaging. By incorporating leaderboards, badges, and virtual rewards, students are motivated to participate actively and retain the financial concepts they learn ( Schöbel et al., 2020 ; Hamari et al., 2014 ).
Emerging technologies like Virtual Reality (VR) and Augmented Reality (AR) are beginning to be used in financial education to create immersive learning experiences. For example, VR simulations can immerse students in virtual scenarios requiring financial decisions, such as navigating a supermarket on a budget or choosing between different financial products, thereby enhancing the realism and applicability of financial concepts ( Georgiou and Kyza, 2018 ).
Financial mentorship programs, pairing students with financial mentors from the professional world, offer real-world insights and guidance. These programs can be particularly beneficial for high school and college students as they prepare to enter the workforce and manage their finances independently ( Lusardi et al., 2017 ).
Policy-driven financial education initiatives in some countries and regions mandate financial education in schools, ensuring that all students receive a basic level of financial education, regardless of their background. This recognition of financial education's importance for economic wellbeing is crucial for creating a uniformly financially literate society ( OECD, 2020 - OECD/INFE 2020 International Survey of Adult Financial Literacy).
Community-based financial challenges, such as savings competitions or budgeting challenges, encourage learning through participation. These programs involve the community in financial education, helping students apply what they've learned in a supportive, community-driven context ( Sherraden et al., 2011 ).
Customized learning pathways recognize the diversity of learners, adapting to each student's knowledge level, interests, and learning pace. This personalized approach ensures that students remain engaged and receive the support they need to advance their financial literacy ( Merry et al., 2022 ).
By leveraging a mix of traditional and innovative approaches, financial education programs can cater to diverse learning needs and preferences, ensuring that students not only learn financial concepts but also develop the skills and confidence to apply them in their lives. Building upon the diverse and innovative approaches to financial education, it is paramount to consider how these programs can adapt to the evolving societal and technological landscapes. The incorporation of cross-cultural financial literacy programs is becoming increasingly essential in our interconnected world. Such programs offer students insights into global financial systems, diverse economic conditions, and the influence of cultural norms on financial behaviors, preparing them for international opportunities and challenges ( Mändmaa, 2019 ).
Similarly, integrating sustainability and ethical finance concepts into financial literacy education addresses the growing concerns about climate change and social inequality. This approach educates young individuals about the environmental and societal impacts of financial decisions, promoting responsible, and sustainable financial behavior ( Thorp et al., 2023 ; Ye and Kulathunga, 2019 ).
The application of adaptive learning technologies in financial education personalizes the learning experience. By analyzing students' learning patterns, these technologies can adjust the difficulty of materials and introduce new topics at the most appropriate times, thus enhancing engagement and learning outcomes ( Peng et al., 2019 ; Nicol et al., 2017 ; Conklin, 2016 ).
As entrepreneurship becomes a more viable career path for many young individuals, financial education programs are incorporating modules specifically designed to teach entrepreneurial finance. This includes lessons on start-up budgeting, venture capital, and financial planning for small businesses ( OECD, 2015 ). Leveraging big data and analytics in financial education allows educators and policymakers to gauge the effectiveness of different teaching methods and programs. This data-driven approach can identify knowledge gaps, tailor programs to meet those needs, and monitor progress over time ( Saleh et al., 2022 ).
Lastly, ensuring financial education programs are accessible to students with diverse abilities is crucial. This involves the use of assistive technologies, the creation of materials in various formats, and the design of inclusive learning activities that accommodate all learners ( Burgstahler, 2020 ).
As financial education continues to evolve, it is vital for educators, policymakers, and communities to remain abreast of best practices, innovative technologies, and emerging needs. Cultivating a culture of lifelong learning and adapting to the changing financial landscape, financial education programs can empower adolescents and young adults with the knowledge and skills necessary for financial wellbeing and success.
Table 5 presents a comprehensive overview of the different types of financial literacy interventions, categorized by the studies that have examined their effectiveness. This table includes details on the age ranges of participants, types of interventions used, and key findings from each study. The interventions highlighted in Table 4 , such as school-based curricula, community challenges, digital platforms, and mentorship programs, demonstrate the diverse approaches used to engage learners and improve financial literacy across different contexts.
Table 5 . Types of financial literacy interventions.
3.4 Effectiveness of financial literacy programs
Financial literacy programs have gained traction as essential tools for enhancing financial knowledge, skills, and attitudes among students and adolescents, ultimately aiming to prepare them for effective financial decision-making in adulthood. These programs typically employ a combination of instructional methods, including classroom teaching, interactive workshops, simulations, and digital learning platforms, to cover fundamental financial concepts such as budgeting, saving, investing, and responsible borrowing ( Amagir et al., 2018 ). However, the effectiveness of these initiatives can vary significantly based on factors such as program design, delivery methods, and the demographic characteristics of participants ( Skimmyhorn, 2016 ; Hati, 2017 ).
One critical factor influencing the success of financial literacy programs is their capacity to engage students through practical applications of financial concepts. Programs that integrate experiential learning components—such as simulations and project-based activities—are often more effective than traditional lecture-based formats. For example, initiatives that allow students to manage virtual budgets or engage in investment simulations can facilitate the translation of theoretical knowledge into practical skills, thereby enhancing overall financial literacy ( Frisancho et al., 2021 ). This aligns with findings that emphasize the importance of active participation in learning processes, which can lead to improved financial behaviors among participants ( Lučić et al., 2020 ).
The timing and duration of financial literacy education also significantly impact its effectiveness. Research indicates that programs integrated into the educational curriculum from an early age and sustained over time yield better outcomes in fostering long-term financial understanding and positive financial behaviors. Early exposure enables students to build foundational knowledge that can be reinforced throughout their educational journey, making them more adept at navigating financial decisions as they mature ( Bhandare et al., 2021 ). Moreover, the relevance of the educational content to the participants' life circumstances is crucial; programs that address the specific challenges faced by different demographic groups, such as low-income students or young women, tend to resonate more effectively with these audiences, enhancing their engagement and retention of financial concepts ( Hati, 2017 ).
Despite the promising outcomes associated with financial literacy programs, several studies have identified limitations that may hinder their overall effectiveness. Issues such as inadequate long-term follow-up, variability in teaching quality, and a lack of integration with broader educational goals can undermine the impact of these initiatives. Programs that focus solely on knowledge dissemination without fostering behavioral change may struggle to effect lasting improvements in participants' financial habits ( Suseno et al., 2021 ; Yin et al., 2022 ). Continuous reinforcement and practical opportunities to apply learned concepts are essential for ensuring that students retain and utilize the financial knowledge acquired through these programs ( Watanapongvanich et al., 2021 ).
Table 6 provides an overview of key studies examining the effectiveness of various financial literacy programs, highlighting the age ranges of participants, key components of the programs, and main findings. The table includes a range of instructional methods, such as classroom teaching, interactive workshops, simulations, and digital platforms, and evaluates how factors like program design, delivery methods, and the demographic characteristics of participants influence outcomes.
Table 6 . Effectiveness of financial literacy programs.
3.5 Factors influencing financial literacy
Research highlights various factors that influence financial literacy among children, adolescents, and young adults, emphasizing the role of financial socialization agents, individual behaviors, and socioeconomic backgrounds. Understanding how children and adolescents comprehend and engage with financial concepts is a crucial area of study that involves various factors. Research has shown that financial literacy among young individuals is influenced by financial socialization agents, financial experiences, and money attitudes ( Sohn et al., 2012 ). Adolescents in economically disadvantaged settings may struggle with grasping complex financial ideas, making it essential to focus on instilling healthy financial behaviors early on ( Zhu, 2019 ). Moreover, the involvement of parents in financial education programs has been found to be effective in enhancing the financial literacy of children and adolescents ( Amagir et al., 2020 ; Van Campenhout, 2015 ).
The family environment plays a significant role in promoting adolescents' financial confidence, indirectly contributing to their financial literacy skills ( Silinskas et al., 2023 ). Parental influence, discussions about money matters, and monitoring children's financial activities can lead to increased financial knowledge and the development of positive financial attitudes and behaviors ( Chawla et al., 2022 ). Financial literacy is crucial as it can mediate the relationship between experiences during adolescence and financial behavior ( Bucciol et al., 2022 ).
Studies conducted in South Korea ( Sohn et al., 2012 ) and Hong Kong ( Zhu et al., 2021 ) emphasize the impact of parental financial behaviors, financial experiences, and money attitudes in shaping financial literacy, particularly for adolescents from low-income families. These findings suggest that parents who model healthy financial practices can significantly enhance their children's financial understanding.
Research from Ethiopia ( Elifneh, 2022 ), Finland ( Silinskas et al., 2021 ), and Japan ( Okamoto and Komamura, 2021 ) consistently reports low levels of financial literacy among adolescents. Factors such as exposure to financial education at school and home, gender differences, and socioeconomic status have been identified as significant influences on financial literacy levels among youth ( Yahiaoui, 2023 ; Gudjonsson et al., 2022 ; Venkatesan and Venkataraman, 2018 ). The influence of family, peers, and individual behaviors on financial literacy has been further highlighted in studies from Saudi Arabia ( Alshebami and Aldhyani, 2022 ), India ( Rani and Goyal, 2021 ), the United Arab Emirates ( Suri and Purohit, 2017 ), and Indonesia ( Lantara and Kartini, 2015 ).
The problem of low financial literacy extends beyond adolescents to young adults and university students, as shown in studies from various countries ( Ninan and Kurian, 2021 ; Amagir et al., 2020 ; Kaur et al., 2015 ). Financial literacy is critical for making informed decisions, managing personal finances, and achieving financial wellbeing ( Chaulagain, 2021 ; Yong and Tan, 2017 ). The relationship between financial literacy, financial behavior, and financial attitudes has been explored across multiple contexts, reinforcing the importance of financial education and practical experience in enhancing financial literacy ( Suhana et al., 2022 ; Yanto et al., 2021 ).
Studies consistently report low levels of financial literacy in the USA ( Lusardi, 2019 ), India ( Kim and Chawla, 2022 ), South Africa ( Nanziri and Leibbrandt, 2018 ), Japan ( Watanapongvanich et al., 2022 ), and Finland ( Silinskas et al., 2021 , 2023 ; Zhu, 2019 ). A positive correlation exists between financial literacy and educational attainment, with higher schooling levels linked to improved financial knowledge ( Lusardi and Mitchell, 2011 ). Parental influence is also crucial, as responsible financial behaviors by parents are associated with higher financial literacy levels in their children ( Zhu and Chou, 2020 ; Desiyanti and Kassim, 2020 ).
Cognitive factors also play a role; reductions in both crystallized and fluid intelligence are linked to declines in financial literacy ( Okamoto and Komamura, 2021 ). Insufficient financial knowledge is associated with risky financial behaviors, such as overspending and impulsive purchases ( Ravikumar et al., 2022 ), while higher financial literacy is linked to improved control over such behaviors ( Potrich and Vieira, 2018 ; Paylan and Kavas, 2022 ).
Financial literacy programs have been shown to provide economic benefits, offering young individuals exposure to basic and advanced financial concepts ( Lusardi and Mitchell, 2014 ; Silinskas et al., 2023 , 2021 ). Parental involvement enhances the effectiveness of these programs, reinforcing financial lessons learned ( Fan et al., 2022 ; Fan and Zhang, 2021 ; Zhu, 2018 ; Van Campenhout, 2015 ). School-based financial education has been shown to improve financial knowledge and attitudes, particularly when incorporating experiential learning approaches that align with students' life events and challenges ( Amagir et al., 2018 ).
Early exposure to financial literacy values significantly improves financial understanding and habits in young people ( Phung, 2023 ; Saputra and Susanti, 2021 ; Clark et al., 2018 ). Tailored educational interventions early in life can strengthen financial literacy and are particularly vital in poverty reduction and economic empowerment in developing countries ( Moscarola and Kalwij, 2021 ; Ansong et al., 2023 ; Koomson et al., 2023 ; Arini et al., 2020 ).
Various demographic, socioeconomic, and psychological factors influence financial literacy, including age, gender, education, income, and parental influence. Socioeconomic and demographic variables such as family income and parental education significantly impact financial literacy levels ( Respati et al., 2023 ; Okamoto and Komamura, 2021 ; Silinskas et al., 2021 ; Tran, 2022 ; Mishra et al., 2021 ; Tavares and Santos, 2020 ; Nicolini and Haupt, 2019 ; Garg and Singh, 2018 ). Psychological factors, such as confidence and motivation, are also crucial, affecting how adolescents perceive and engage with financial education ( Silinskas et al., 2023 ; Khawar and Sarwar, 2021 ; Tang and Baker, 2016 ). External factors, including cultural preferences and behavioral biases, further influence financial literacy ( Kalmi et al., 2021 ). Education level plays a significant role, with higher educational attainment linked to better financial literacy, and gender differences suggesting that financial literacy varies between boys and girls ( Mishra et al., 2021 ; Tavares and Santos, 2020 ; Mancebón et al., 2019 ). Overall, understanding the factors that influence financial literacy helps educators and policymakers design effective interventions that address these diverse influences and foster financial competence among young individuals.
Table 7 presents an overview of key studies that investigate the factors affecting financial literacy in children, adolescents, and young adults. The table highlights the age ranges of participants, specific factors explored, and the main findings of each study. These studies underscore the importance of early financial socialization, the role of family and peers, and the impact of socioeconomic backgrounds on financial knowledge and behaviors.
Table 7 . Factors influencing financial literacy.
4 Discussion
The reviewed literature on financial literacy programs highlights the need for educators to employ experiential learning methods and life-event-focused content to enhance the effectiveness of financial education. Financial literacy encompasses the multifaceted ability to understand and effectively apply financial skills, including personal financial management, budgeting, and investing. This concept is crucial for informed decision-making, allowing individuals to navigate financial situations wisely. Lusardi and Tufano (2015) and Lusardi and Mitchell (2014) stress its importance for economic empowerment and the avoidance of financial pitfalls through an understanding of complex financial products and risk management. Huston (2010) further links financial literacy to behaviors and attitudes toward money, suggesting that a proactive financial stance and a willingness to learn are key to sound financial decisions. Swiecka et al. (2020) discuss its role in preventing financial distress and enhancing economic wellbeing. This comprehensive view underscores financial literacy as vital for achieving financial security and underscores the need for educational initiatives to build financial knowledge, skills, and attitudes from a young age, fostering a financially informed society.
Financial literacy is a comprehensive concept that includes knowledge, skills, attitudes, and behaviors necessary for making well-informed financial decisions and effectively managing personal finances. Goyal and Kumar (2021) emphasize that financial literacy not only involves understanding financial concepts but also the ability to apply this knowledge in practical financial decision-making. In the current digital era, the incorporation of technology in financial transactions presents opportunities and challenges, underscoring the need for updated financial literacy frameworks that encompass digital financial services and cybersecurity awareness.
Monteith et al. (2021) highlight the increasing worries about cybercrime, which have been worsened by the epidemic. They emphasize the importance of improving cybersecurity awareness to protect individuals from financial and psychological consequences. Emotional intelligence is increasingly acknowledged for its influence on financial decision-making, as emotions can greatly affect financial actions. Alshebami and Al Marri (2022) examine how financial literacy affects entrepreneurial intention, emphasizing the significance of saving behavior in financial decision-making.
The importance of emotional intelligence in financial decision-making is increasingly recognized, shedding light on the psychological aspects of financial behavior ( Sohn et al., 2022 ). By expanding financial literacy to include emotional dimensions, individuals can embrace a more holistic approach to financial education, preparing them to make well-informed financial decisions in a complex digital environment ( Krichen and Chaabouni, 2022 ; Nurlaily et al., 2022 ).
Understanding the pivotal role of financial literacy across the spectrum of life stages opens a crucial dialogue on its evolution from youth through adulthood to the golden years. This progression underscores the necessity of a tailored, life-cycle approach to financial education, highlighting the importance of equipping individuals with the knowledge and skills pertinent to their current phase of life ( Diotaiuti et al., 2020 , 2021a , b ). By integrating real-world financial applications, ranging from tax planning to retirement and estate planning, this approach not only enhances the relevance of financial literacy but also prepares individuals to navigate the complexities of financial decision-making throughout their lives, thus cultivating a society that is both resilient and financially informed. For policymakers, it underscores the importance of supporting and mandating comprehensive financial education within school curriculums to ensure young individuals develop necessary financial skills ( LeBaron-Black et al., 2023 ; LeBaron and Kelley, 2021 ). Parents play a crucial role in reinforcing financial literacy at home, suggesting a collaborative approach involving schools and families is essential for cultivating financially savvy individuals. These findings advocate for integrated efforts among educators, policymakers, and parents to optimize financial literacy outcomes for children and adolescents ( Zhao and Zhang, 2020 ; Pahlevan Sharif and Naghavi, 2020 ).
Incorporating real-life financial scenarios and experiential learning into educational content significantly enhances the effectiveness of these programs. This approach can better prepare students for the financial decisions they will face in adulthood ( Emma and Anya, 2021 ). Policymakers are prompted to consider these findings when developing educational policies, ensuring that financial literacy is an integral part of the curriculum. For parents, the literature suggests that their active involvement in their children's financial education can profoundly influence their financial behaviors and attitudes, reinforcing the importance of a collaborative effort between schools and families in financial education ( Đurišić and Bunijevac, 2017 ). This integrated approach among educators, policymakers, and parents can significantly contribute to the development of a financially literate generation.
For educators, incorporating practical financial activities and digital tools in the curriculum can make learning more engaging and relevant. Policymakers should focus on creating and supporting policies that integrate financial literacy into national education standards, ensuring consistent access across all schools ( Böhm et al., 2023 ; Sagita et al., 2022 ; Poon et al., 2022 ; Salas-Velasco et al., 2021 ). For parents, actively participating in their children's financial education through discussions and practical financial decision-making activities at home is crucial. These actions can collectively contribute to a robust financial literacy foundation, preparing young individuals for future financial independence and responsibility ( Pahlevan Sharif and Naghavi, 2020 ).
Identifying challenges in teaching financial literacy to young populations includes addressing diverse learning needs, engaging students with relevant and practical content, and integrating financial education effectively within existing curricula. Additionally, limitations in current research often involve a lack of longitudinal studies to measure the long-term impact of financial literacy programs and a need for more comprehensive data on the effectiveness of different teaching methodologies across diverse demographic groups. These challenges and research gaps highlight the need for tailored educational approaches and further investigation into the outcomes of financial literacy education ( Henderson et al., 2021 ).
Further challenges in teaching financial literacy to young populations include maintaining the relevance of financial education in the face of rapidly changing financial landscapes and technologies. There's also the difficulty of engaging parents and communities in financial education efforts, which is crucial for reinforcing learning outside the classroom. In terms of research limitations, there's a need for more culturally sensitive studies that take into account the socioeconomic diversity among learners. Additionally, much of the existing research relies on self-reported data, which may not accurately reflect actual financial behavior or literacy ( Kuzma et al., 2022 ; Huebinger et al., 2021 ; Al-Bahrani et al., 2019 ).
In order to enhance financial education strategies, practical suggestions include creating more interactive and engaging learning experiences through simulations and gamification, ensuring content relevance by incorporating current financial trends and technologies, and fostering partnerships between educational institutions, financial experts, and communities to provide real-world learning opportunities. Tailoring financial literacy programs to meet the diverse needs of students and incorporating feedback mechanisms for continuous improvement are also key.
Future research in financial education should explore the long-term impacts of financial literacy programs through longitudinal studies, assessing how early financial education influences adult financial behaviors and wellbeing. Investigating the role of digital financial tools and their effectiveness in enhancing financial literacy among diverse populations can also provide valuable insights. Additionally, examining the socio-economic factors that influence financial education outcomes and developing tailored strategies to address these variables could significantly contribute to the field. Further areas for investigation include the efficacy of financial literacy education across different cultural contexts, understanding the psychological barriers to financial behavior change, and the impact of emerging technologies like blockchain on financial education. Research could also explore the integration of financial literacy with environmental, social, and governance (ESG) principles to promote sustainable financial decisions among young people.
5 Limits of the review
The review's limitations include its scope, focusing primarily on studies that measure immediate outcomes of financial literacy programs without extensive exploration of long-term behavioral changes. Additionally, the diversity of methodologies and populations in the reviewed studies may impact the generalizability of the findings. Future research could benefit from addressing these gaps by incorporating longitudinal studies and a broader range of socio-economic and cultural contexts to provide a more comprehensive understanding of financial literacy education's effectiveness.
6 Conclusions
The main findings emphasize that financial literacy programs are crucial in enhancing financial knowledge and behavior among children and adolescents. Effective strategies include experiential learning, integration of real-life financial scenarios, and parental involvement. Challenges exist, such as adapting to rapid technological changes and addressing diverse learning needs. Future research should focus on longitudinal studies to assess long-term impacts, the role of digital tools, and socio-economic influences on financial education outcomes. Addressing these areas is key to developing more inclusive and impactful financial literacy education.
Comprehensive, age-appropriate financial education plays a pivotal role in nurturing financially literate and capable adults. By starting early and tailoring lessons to fit age-specific understanding and interests, such education lays the foundation for sound financial decision-making. It equips young people with the necessary skills to navigate complex financial landscapes, ensuring they grow into adults who can manage finances effectively, plan for the future, and respond adaptively to economic challenges. This approach fosters a generation that is not only financially savvy but also prepared for lifelong financial wellbeing.
Author contributions
SM: Conceptualization, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. BT: Conceptualization, Investigation, Writing – original draft. SC: Conceptualization, Investigation, Writing – review & editing. GS: Investigation, Writing – review & editing. AZ: Conceptualization, Investigation, Writing – review & editing. PD: Supervision, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing.
The author(s) declare that no financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.
Conflict of interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Publisher's note
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
Đurišić, M., and Bunijevac, M. (2017). Parental involvement as a important factor for successful education. Center Educ. Policy Stud. J. 7, 137–153. doi: 10.26529/cepsj.291
Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar
Al-Bahrani, A., Weathers, J., and Patel, D. (2019). Racial differences in the returns to financial literacy education. J. Cons. Affairs 53, 572–599. doi: 10.1111/joca.12205
Alshebami, A. S., and Al Marri, S. H. (2022). The impact of financial literacy on entrepreneurial intention: the mediating role of saving behavior. Front. Psychol. 13:911605. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.911605
PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar
Alshebami, A. S., and Aldhyani, T. H. (2022). The interplay of social influence, financial literacy, and saving behaviour among Saudi youth and the moderating effect of self-control. Sustainability 14:8780. doi: 10.3390/su14148780
Amagir, A., Groot, W., Maassen van den Brink, H., and Wilschut, A. (2018). A review of financial-literacy education programs for children and adolescents. Citizenship, Soc. Econ. Educ. 17, 56–80. doi: 10.1177/2047173417719555
Amagir, A., Groot, W., Maassen van den Brink, H., and Wilschut, A. (2019). SaveWise: the design of a financial education program in the Netherlands. Citizenship, Soc. Econ. Educ. 18, 100–120. doi: 10.1177/2047173419870053
Amagir, A., Groot, W., van den Brink, H. M., and Wilschut, A. (2020). Financial literacy of high school students in the Netherlands: knowledge, attitudes, self-efficacy, and behavior. Int. Rev. Econ. Educ. 34:100185. doi: 10.1016/j.iree.2020.100185
Ansong, D., Okumu, M., Nyoni, T., Appiah-Kubi, J., Amoako, E. O., Koomson, I., et al. (2023). The effectiveness of financial capability and asset building interventions in improving youth's educational well-being: a systematic review. Adol. Res. Rev. 2023, 1–16. doi: 10.1007/s40894-023-00223-x
Arini, F. D., Widaningrum, A., and Hadna, A. H. (2020). “Poverty reduction and financial literacy for women in Indonesia,” in Annual Conference of Indonesian Association for Public Administration (IAPA 2019) (Atlantis Press), 241–258. doi: 10.2991/aebmr.k.200301.013
Arofah, A. A., Purwaningsih, Y., and Indriayu, M. (2018). Financial literacy, materialism and financial behavior. Int. J. Multic. Multirel. Underst. 5, 370–378. doi: 10.18415/ijmmu.v5i4.171
Bandura, A. (1977). Self-efficacy: toward a unifying theory of behavioral change. Psychol. Rev. 84, 191–215. doi: 10.1037//0033-295X.84.2.191
Barrot, J. S., Gonzales, J. M., Eniego, A. A., Salipande, A. L., and Olegario, M. L. G. (2024). Integrating financial literacy into the K-12 curriculum: teachers' and school leaders' experience. Asia-Pacific Educ. Resear. 33, 17–25. doi: 10.1007/s40299-022-00704-y
Batty, M., Collins, J. M., and Odders-White, E. (2015). Experimental evidence on the effects of financial education on elementary school students' knowledge, behavior, and attitudes. J. Cons. Affairs 49, 69–96. doi: 10.1111/joca.12058
Bhandare, P., Guha, S., Chaudhury, R. H., and Ghosh, C. (2021). Impact of financial literacy models on the financial behavior of individuals: an empirical study on the indian context. Strat. Change 30, 377–387. doi: 10.1002/jsc.2431
Böhm, P., Böhmová, G., Gazdíková, J., and Šimková, V. (2023). Determinants of financial literacy: analysis of the impact of family and socioeconomic variables on undergraduate students in the Slovak republic. J. Risk Finan. Manag. 16:252. doi: 10.3390/jrfm16040252
Bucciol, A., Manfr,è, M., and Veronesi, M. (2022). Family financial socialization and wealth decisions. BE J. Econ. Anal. Policy 22, 281–309. doi: 10.1515/bejeap-2021-0065
Burgstahler, S. (2020). Creating Inclusive Learning Opportunities in Higher Education: A Universal Design Toolkit . Cambridge, MA: Harvard Education Press, 47–48.
Google Scholar
Caplinska, A., and Ohotina, A. (2019). Analysis of financial literacy tendencies with young people. Entrepr. Sustain. Issues 6, 1736–1749. doi: 10.9770/jesi.2019.6.4(13)
Chaulagain, R. (2021). Relationship among financial literacy, attitude and behaviour. Soc. Inquir. 3, 10–29. doi: 10.3126/sijssr.v3i1.46017
Chawla, D., Bhatia, S., and Singh, S. (2022). Parental influence, financial literacy and investment behaviour of young adults. J. Indian Bus. Res. 14, 520–539. doi: 10.1108/JIBR-10-2021-0357
Chhatwani, M., and Mishra, S. K. (2021). Does financial literacy reduce financial fragility during COVID-19? The moderation effect of psychological, economic and social factors. Int. J. Bank Market. 39, 1114–1133. doi: 10.1108/IJBM-11-2020-0536
Clark, S., Paul, M., Aryeetey, R., and Marquis, G. (2018). An assets-based approach to promoting girls' financial literacy, savings, and education. J. Adolesc. 68, 94–104. doi: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2018.07.010
Collins, J. M., and Odders-White, E. (2015). A framework for developing and testing financial capability education programs targeted to elementary schools. J. Econ. Educ. 46, 105–120. doi: 10.1080/00220485.2014.976325
Compen, B., De Witte, K., and Schelfhout, W. (2019). The role of teacher professional development in financial literacy education: a systematic literature review. Educ. Res. Rev. 26, 16–31. doi: 10.1016/j.edurev.2018.12.001
Conklin, T. A. (2016). Knewton (An adaptive learning platform available at: https://www.knewton.com/ ). Acad. Manag. Lear. Educ. 15:206. doi: 10.5465/amle.2016.0206
Cordero, J. M., and Pedraja, F. (2019). The effect of financial education training on the financial literacy of Spanish students in PISA. Appl. Econ. 51, 1679–1693. doi: 10.1080/00036846.2018.1528336
Desiyanti, R., and Kassim, A. A. M. (2020). Financial literacy among SMEs' owners in sumatera, Indonesia: the role of parents' motivation and experience. J. Bus. Soc. Rev. Emer. Econ. 6, 1130–1138. doi: 10.26710/jbsee.v6i3.1374
Diotaiuti, P., Mancone, S., Falese, L., Ferrara, M., Bellizzi, F., Valente, G., et al. (2022a). Intention to screen for Hepatitis C among university students: influence of different communicative scenarios. Front. Psychiatry 13:873566. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2022.873566
Diotaiuti, P., Valente, G., and Mancone, S. (2021b). Validation study of the Italian version of Temporal Focus Scale: psychometric properties and convergent validity. BMC Psychol. 9:19. doi: 10.1186/s40359-021-00561-2
Diotaiuti, P., Valente, G., Mancone, S., and Bellizzi, F. (2021a). A mediating model of emotional balance and procrastination on academic performance. Front. Psychol. 12:665196. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.665196
Diotaiuti, P., Valente, G., Mancone, S., and Grambone, A. (2020). Psychometric properties and a preliminary validation study of the Italian brief version of the communication styles inventory (CSI-B/I). Front. Psychol. 11:1421. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01421
Diotaiuti, P., Valente, G., Mancone, S., Grambone, A., Chirico, A., and Lucidi, F. (2022b). The use of the Decision Regret Scale in non-clinical contexts. Front. Psychol. 13:945669. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.945669
Elifneh, Y. W. (2022). “Financial literacy among Ethiopian high school teenagers,” in Business, Industry, and Trade in the Tropics (Routledge), 196–213. doi: 10.4324/9781003153580-11
Emma, M., and Anya, S. (2021). A study on financial literacy and need for financial education. J. Enter. Bus. Intell. 1, 137–142. doi: 10.53759/5181/JEBI202101015
Eniola, A. A., and Entebang, H. (2017). SME managers and financial literacy. Global Business Rev. 18, 559–576. doi: 10.1177/0972150917692063
Fan, L., Lim, H., and Lee, J. M. (2022). Young adults' financial advice-seeking behavior: the roles of parental financial socialization. Fam. Relat. 71, 1226–1246. doi: 10.1111/fare.12625
Fan, L., and Zhang, L. (2021). The influence of financial education sources on emergency savings: the role of financial literacy. Family Consumer Sci. Res. J. 49, 344–361. doi: 10.1111/fcsr.12400
Fauziaha, P., and Sari, R. C. (2019). The development of a financial literacy questionnaire for early childhood. Development 7, 305–315.
Frisancho, V. (2020). The impact of financial education for youth. Econ. Educ. Rev. 78:101918. doi: 10.1016/j.econedurev.2019.101918
Frisancho, V., Herrera, A., and Prina, S. (2021). Can a budget-recording tool teach financial skills to youth? Experimental evidence from a financial diaries study. IDB Working Paper Series. doi: 10.18235/0003691
Fuente-Mella, H. D. L., Umaña-Hermosilla, B., Fonseca-Fuentes, M., and Elórtegui-Gómez, C. (2021). Multinomial logistic regression to estimate the financial education and financial knowledge of university students in Chile. Information 12:379. doi: 10.3390/info12090379
Gardynia, N., and Syaodih, E. (2021). “Learning cycle for literacy financial of early childhood education,” in ICOPE 2020: Proceedings of the 2nd International Conference on Progressive Education, ICOPE 2020, 16-17 October 2020, Universitas Lampung, Bandar Lampung, Indonesia (European Alliance for Innovation), 453. doi: 10.4108/eai.16-10-2020.2305227
Garg, N., and Singh, S. (2018). Financial literacy among youth. Int. J. Soc. Econ. 45, 173–186. doi: 10.1108/IJSE-11-2016-0303
Georgiou, Y., and Kyza, E. A. (2018). Relations between student motivation, immersion and learning outcomes in location-based augmented reality settings. Comput. Hum. Behav. 89, 173–181. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2018.08.011
Goyal, K., and Kumar, S. (2021). Financial literacy: a systematic review and bibliometric analysis. Int. J. Consum. Stud. 45, 80–105. doi: 10.1111/ijcs.12605
Gudjonsson, S., Minelgaite, I., Kristinsson, K., and Pálsdóttir, S. (2022). Financial literacy and gender differences: women choose people while men choose things? Admin. Sci. 12:179. doi: 10.3390/admsci12040179
Hamari, J., Koivisto, J., and Sarsa, H. (2014). “Does gamification work?–a literature review of empirical studies on gamification,” in 2014 47th Hawaii International Conference on System Sciences (IEEE), 3025–3034. doi: 10.1109/HICSS.2014.377
Hati, S. R. H. (2017). Exploring the motivation toward and perceived usefulness of a financial education: program offered to low-income women in indonesia. ASEAN J. Commun. Engag. 1:17. doi: 10.7454/vol1iss1pp17-28
Henderson, G. E., Beach, P., and Coombs, A. (2021). Financial literacy education in Ontario: an exploratory study of elementary teachers' perceptions, attitudes, and practices. Canad. J. Educ. 44, 308–336. doi: 10.53967/cje-rce.v44i2.4249
Huebinger, R. M., Hussain, R., Tupchong, K., Walia, S., Fairbrother, H., and Rogg, J. (2021). Survey-based evaluation of resident and attending financial literacy. Western J. Emer. Med. 22:1369. doi: 10.5811/westjem.2021.8.53016
Hussain, J., Salia, S., and Karim, A. (2018). Is knowledge that powerful? Financial literacy and access to finance: an analysis of enterprises in the UK. J. Small Bus. Enter. Dev. 25, 985–1003. doi: 10.1108/JSBED-01-2018-0021
Huston, S. J. (2010). Measuring financial literacy. J. Consumer Affairs 44, 296–316. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-6606.2010.01170.x
Indratirta, N. P., Handayati, P., and Juliardi, D. (2023). Effect of financial literacy on the financial behavior of the millennial generation about the dangers of fraudulent investment and flexing affiliations. J. Bus. Manag. Econ. Dev. 1, 355–365. doi: 10.59653/jbmed.v1i02.154
Johnson, J., Spraggon, D., Stevenson, G., Levine, E., and Mancari, G. (2021). Impact of the futuresmart online financial education course on financial knowledge of middle school students. J. Finan. Couns. Plan. 32, 368–386. doi: 10.1891/JFCP-19-00061
Jonker, N., and Kosse, A. (2022). The interplay of financial education, financial inclusion and financial stability and the role of Big Tech. Contemp. Econ. Policy 40, 612–635. doi: 10.1111/coep.12578
Kadoya, Y., and Khan, M. S. R. (2020). What determines financial literacy in Japan? J. Pension Econ. Finance 19, 353–371. doi: 10.1017/S1474747218000379
Kaiser, T., Lusardi, A., Menkhoff, L., and Urban, C. (2022). Financial education affects financial knowledge and downstream behaviors. J. Financ. Econ. 145, 255–272. doi: 10.1016/j.jfineco.2021.09.022
Kaiser, T., and Menkhoff, L. (2020). Financial education in schools: A meta-analysis of experimental studies. Econ. Educ. Rev. 78:101930. doi: 10.1016/j.econedurev.2019.101930
Kalmi, P., Trotta, G., and KaŽukauskas, A. (2021). Energy-related financial literacy and electricity consumption: survey-based evidence from Finland. J. Consumer Affairs 55, 1062–1089. doi: 10.1111/joca.12395
Kalwij, A., Alessie, R., Dinkova, M., Schonewille, G., Van der Schors, A., and Van der Werf, M. (2019). The effects of financial education on financial literacy and savings behavior: evidence from a controlled field experiment in Dutch primary schools. J. Consumer Affairs 53, 699–730. doi: 10.1111/joca.12241
Kaur, M., Vohra, T., and Arora, A. (2015). Financial literacy among university students: a study of Guru Nanak Dev University, Amritsar, Punjab. Asia-Pacific J. Manag. Res. Innov. 11, 143–152. doi: 10.1177/2319510X15576178
Khawar, S., and Sarwar, A. (2021). Financial literacy and financial behavior with the mediating effect of family financial socialization in the financial institutions of Lahore, Pakistan. Fut. Bus. J. 7, 1–11. doi: 10.1186/s43093-021-00064-x
Kim, J., and Chawla, I. (2022). 26 The Role of Socialization in Shaping Personal Finance Attitudes and Behaviors. Berlin: De Gruyter Handbook of Personal Finance 451. doi: 10.1515/9783110727692-026
Klapper, L., and Lusardi, A. (2020). Financial literacy and financial resilience: evidence from around the world. Financ. Manag. 49, 589–614. doi: 10.1111/fima.12283
Koomson, I., Villano, R. A., and Hadley, D. (2023). The role of financial literacy in households' asset accumulation process: Evidence from Ghana. Rev. Econ. Househ. 21, 591–614. doi: 10.1007/s11150-022-09603-z
Krichen, K., and Chaabouni, H. (2022). Entrepreneurial intention of academic students in the time of COVID-19 pandemic. J. Small Bus. Enter. Dev. 29, 106–126. doi: 10.1108/JSBED-03-2021-0110
Kuzma, I., Chaikovska, H., Levchyk, I., and Yankovych, O. (2022). Formation of financial literacy in primary school students. J. Effic. Respons. Educ. Sci. 15, 142–155. doi: 10.7160/eriesj.2022.150302
Lantara, I. W. N., and Kartini, N. K. R. (2015). Financial literacy among university students: empirical evidence from Indonesia. J. Indonesian Econ. Bus. 30:247. doi: 10.22146/jieb.10314
LeBaron, A. B., and Kelley, H. H. (2021). Financial socialization: a decade in review. J. Fam. Econ. Issues 42, 195–206. doi: 10.1007/s10834-020-09736-2
LeBaron-Black, A. B., Curran, M. A., Hill, E. J., Freeh, M. E., Toomey, R. B., and Speirs, K. E. (2022). Parent financial socialization scale: development and preliminary validation. J. Family Psychol. 36:943. doi: 10.1037/fam0000927
LeBaron-Black, A. B., Curran, M. A., Hill, E. J., Toomey, R. B., Speirs, K. E., and Freeh, M. E. (2023). Talk is cheap: Parent financial socialization and emerging adult financial well-being. Fam. Relat. 72, 1201–1219. doi: 10.1111/fare.12751
Letkiewicz, J. C., and Fox, J. J. (2014). Conscientiousness, financial literacy, and asset accumulation of young adults. J. Consumer Affairs 48, 274–300. doi: 10.1111/joca.12040
Letkiewicz, J. C., Lim, H., Heckman, S. J., and Montalto, C. P. (2019). Parental financial socialization: is too much help leading to debt ignorance among college students? Family Cons. Sci. Res. J. 48, 149–164. doi: 10.1111/fcsr.12341
Liu, C. L., Chang, D. F., and Chuang, C. M. (2019). Investigation of the financial literacy among high school students. ICIC Expr. Lett. Part B 10, 387–393. doi: 10.24507/icicelb.10.05.387
Lučić, A., Barbić, D., and Uzelac, M. (2020). The role of financial education in adolescent consumers' financial knowledge enhancement. Market-TrŽište 32, 115–130. doi: 10.22598/mt/2020.32.spec-issue.115
Lührmann, M., Serra-Garcia, M., and Winter, J. (2015). Teaching teenagers in finance: does it work? J. Bank. Fin. 54, 160–174. doi: 10.1016/j.jbankfin.2014.11.009
Lührmann, M., Serra-Garcia, M., and Winter, J. (2018). The impact of financial education on adolescents' intertemporal choices. Am. Econ. J. 10, 309–332. doi: 10.1257/pol.20170012
Lusardi, A. (2019). Financial literacy and the need for financial education: evidence and implications. Swiss J. Econ. Stat. 155, 1–8. doi: 10.1186/s41937-019-0027-5
Lusardi, A., and Mitchell, O. S. (2011). Financial literacy around the world: an overview. J. Pension Econ. Finance 10, 497–508. doi: 10.1017/S1474747211000448
Lusardi, A., and Mitchell, O. S. (2014). The economic importance of financial literacy: theory and evidence. Am. Econ. J. 52, 5–44. doi: 10.1257/jel.52.1.5
Lusardi, A., Samek, A., Kapteyn, A., Glinert, L., Hung, A., and Heinberg, A. (2017). Visual tools and narratives: new ways to improve financial literacy. J. Pension Econ. Finance 16, 297–323. doi: 10.1017/S1474747215000323
Lusardi, A., and Tufano, P. (2015). Debt literacy, financial experiences, and overindebtedness. J. Pension Econ. Finance 14, 332–368. doi: 10.1017/S1474747215000232
Mahoney, J. L., Weissberg, R. P., Greenberg, M. T., Dusenbury, L., Jagers, R. J., Niemi, K., et al. (2021). Systemic social and emotional learning: promoting educational success for all preschool to high school students. Am. Psychol. 76:1128. doi: 10.1037/amp0000701
Mallia, L., Chirico, A., Zelli, A., Galli, F., Palombi, T., Bortoli, L., et al. (2020). The implementation and evaluation of a media literacy intervention about PAES use in sport science students. Front. Psychol. 11:368. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00368
Mancebón, M. J., Ximénez-de-Embún, D. P., Mediavilla, M., and Gómez-Sancho, J. M. (2019). Factors that influence the financial literacy of young Spanish consumers. Int. J. Consum. Stud. 43, 227–235. doi: 10.1111/ijcs.12502
Mancone, S., Corrado, S., Beatrice, T., Spica, G., and Diotaiuti, P. (2024b). Integrating digital and interactive approaches in adolescent health literacy: a comprehensive review. Front. Public Health 12:1387874. doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2024.1387874
Mancone, S., Corrado, S., Tosti, B., Spica, G., Di Siena, F., Misiti, F., et al. (2024a). Enhancing nutritional knowledge and self-regulation among adolescents: efficacy of a multifaceted food literacy intervention. Front. Psychol. 15:1405414. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2024.1405414
Mändmaa, S. (2019). Financial literacy – what and why should we improve. Eurasian J. Soc. Sci. 7, 12–28. doi: 10.15604/ejss.2019.07.02.002
Marchetti, A., Rinaldi, T., Lombardi, E., Massaro, D., and Valle, A. (2021). “Learning to wait, be altruistic, and fair: a primary school training in economic education,” in Financial Education and Risk Literacy, Behavioural Financial Regulation and Policy (BEFAIRLY) series (Cheltenham, UK: Edward Elgar Publishing), 112–122. doi: 10.4337/9781789908855.00014
Merry, K. E., Webster, F. R., and Kucharczyk, S. (2022). Investing in students with extensive support needs: steps to integrate personal financial literacy in inclusive settings for educators, students, and families. Inclusive Pract. 1, 156–170. doi: 10.1177/27324745221128931
Mishra, D. K., Malik, S., Chitnis, A., Paul, D., and Dash, S. S. (2021). Factors contributing to financial literacy and financial inclusion among women in Indian SHGs. Univ. J. Account. Finance 9, 810–819. doi: 10.13189/ujaf.2021.090427
Monteith, S., Bauer, M., Alda, M., Geddes, J., Whybrow, P. C., and Glenn, T. (2021). Increasing cybercrime since the pandemic: concerns for psychiatry. Curr. Psychiatry Rep. 23, 1–9. doi: 10.1007/s11920-021-01228-w
Moscarola, F. C., and Kalwij, A. (2021). The effectiveness of a formal financial education program at primary schools and the role of informal financial education. Eval. Rev. 45, 107–133. doi: 10.1177/0193841X211042515
Nanziri, E. L., and Leibbrandt, M. (2018). Measuring and profiling financial literacy in South Africa. South African J. Econ. Manag. Sci. 21, 1–17. doi: 10.4102/sajems.v21i1.1645
Nicol, A. A. M., Owens, S. M., Coze, S. S. C. L. L., MacIntyre, A., and Eastwood, C. (2017). Comparison of high-technology active learning and low-technology active learning classrooms. Active Learn. Higher Educ. 19, 253–265. doi: 10.1177/1469787417731176
Nicolini, G., and Haupt, M. (2019). The assessment of financial literacy: new evidence from Europe. Int. J. Finan. Stud. 7:54. doi: 10.3390/ijfs7030054
Ninan, M., and Kurian, A. (2021). A study on the impact of financial literacy on the financial behaviour of college students. Int. J. Innov. Res. Eng. Multidisc. Phys. Sci. 9, 17–22. doi: 10.37082/IJIRMPS.2021.v09i04.003
Nurlaily, F., Asmoro, P. S., and Aini, E. K. (2022). Digital financial literacy and personal financial health during the Covid-19 pandemic. J. MEBIS 7, 54–59. doi: 10.33005/mebis.v7i1.236
OECD (2015). National Strategies for Financial Education . Paris: OECD Publishing.
OECD (2020). OECD/INFE 2020 International Survey of Adult Financial Literacy . Paris: OECD Publishing.
Okamoto, S., and Komamura, K. (2021). Age, gender, and financial literacy in Japan. PLoS ONE 16:e0259393. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0259393
Pahlevan Sharif, S., and Naghavi, N. (2020). Family financial socialization, financial information seeking behavior and financial literacy among youth. Asia-Pacific J. Bus. Admin. 12, 163–181. doi: 10.1108/APJBA-09-2019-0196
Paylan, M. A., and Kavas, Y. B. (2022). A study on the mediating effect of financial literacy between personality traits and irrational buying behaviors. Cumhur. Üniver. Iktisadi Idari Bilimler Dergisi 23, 736–746. doi: 10.37880/cumuiibf.1092137
Peng, H., Ma, S., and Spector, J. M. (2019). Personalized adaptive learning: an emerging pedagogical approach enabled by a smart learning environment. Smart Lear. Environ. 6, 1–14. doi: 10.1186/s40561-019-0089-y
Perdanasari, A., Sudiyanto, S., and Octoria, D. (2019). The importance of financial literacy knowledge for elementary school students in 21st Century. Efektor 6, 26–31. doi: 10.29407/e.v6i1.12591
Permana, W. P., Nandiyanto, A. B. D., Kurniawan, T., and Bilad, M. R. (2021). Financial literature education to increase saving motivation in elementary school students. Indonesian J. Multidic. Res. 1, 385–392. doi: 10.17509/ijomr.v1i2.38564
Phung, T. M. (2023). Parental roles, financial literacy and budgeting behaviour: a survey during the COVID-19 pandemic. J. Appl. Res. Higher Educ. 15, 796–812. doi: 10.1108/JARHE-03-2022-0086
Piaget, J. (1952). The Origins of Intelligence in Children . Madison: International Universities Press. doi: 10.1037/11494-000
Poon, E., Bissonnette, P., Sedighi, S., MacNevin, W., and Kulkarni, K. (2022). Improving financial literacy using the medical mini-MBA at a canadian medical school. Cureus 14:e25595. doi: 10.7759/cureus.25595
Popovich, J. J., Loibl, C., Zirkle, C., and Whittington, M. S. (2020). Community college students' response to a financial literacy intervention: an exploratory study. Int. Rev. Econ. Educ. 34:100182. doi: 10.1016/j.iree.2020.100182
Potrich, A. C. G., and Vieira, K. M. (2018). Demystifying financial literacy: a behavioral perspective analysis. Manag. Res. Rev. 41, 1047–1068. doi: 10.1108/MRR-08-2017-0263
Rani, S., and Goyal, N. (2021). Gender gap in financial literacy: literature review. J. Tianjin Univ. Sci. Technol. 54, 339–354. doi: 10.17605/OSF.IO/SF8MA
Ravikumar, T., Suresha, B., Prakash, N., Vazirani, K., and Krishna, T. A. (2022). Digital financial literacy among adults in India: measurement and validation. Cogent Econ. Finance 10:2132631. doi: 10.1080/23322039.2022.2132631
Respati, D. K., Widyastuti, U., Nuryati, T., Musyaffi, A. M., Handayani, B. D., and Ali, N. R. (2023). How do students' digital financial literacy and financial confidence influence their financial behavior and financial well-being? Nurture 17, 40–50. doi: 10.55951/nurture.v17i2.154
Sagita, L., Putri, R. I. I., and Prahmana, R. C. I. (2022). Promising research studies between mathematics literacy and financial literacy through project-based learning. J. Mathem. Educ. 13, 753–772. doi: 10.22342/jme.v13i4.pp753-772
Salas-Velasco, M., Moreno-Herrero, D., and Sánchez-Campillo, J. (2021). Teaching financial education in schools and students' financial literacy: a cross-country analysis with PISA data. Int. J. Finance Econ. 26, 4077–4103. doi: 10.1002/ijfe.2005
Saleh, I., Marei, Y., Ayoush, M., and Afifa, M. A. (2022). Big data analytics and financial reporting quality: qualitative evidence from canada. J. Financ. Repor. Account. 21, 83–104. doi: 10.1108/JFRA-12-2021-0489
Saputra, J., and Susanti, D. (2021). A study of several financial literacy teaching methods for children. Int. J. Ethno-Sci. Educ. Res. 1, 7–10. doi: 10.46336/ijeer.v1i2.120
Sari, R. C., Aisyah, M. N., Ilyana, S., and Hermawan, H. D. (2022). Developing a financial literacy storybook for early childhood in an augmented reality context. Contemp. Educ. Technol. 14:ep363. doi: 10.30935/cedtech/11734
Schöbel, S. M., Janson, A., and Söllner, M. (2020). Capturing the complexity of gamification elements: a holistic approach for analysing existing and deriving novel gamification designs. Eur. J. Inf. Syst. 29, 641–668. doi: 10.1080/0960085X.2020.1796531
Sherraden, M. S., Johnson, L., Guo, B., and Elliott, W. (2011). Financial capability in children: Effects of participation in a school-based financial education and savings program. J. Fam. Econ. Issues 32, 385–399. doi: 10.1007/s10834-010-9220-5
Silinskas, G., Ahonen, A. K., and Wilska, T. A. (2021). Financial literacy among Finnish adolescents in PISA 2018: the role of financial learning and dispositional factors. Large-scale Assess. Educ. 9, 1–23. doi: 10.1186/s40536-021-00118-0
Silinskas, G., Ahonen, A. K., and Wilska, T. A. (2023). School and family environments promote adolescents' financial confidence: Indirect paths to financial literacy skills in Finnish PISA 2018. J. Consumer Affairs 57, 593–618. doi: 10.1111/joca.12513
Skimmyhorn, W. (2016). Assessing financial education: evidence from boot camp. Am. Econ. J. 8, 322–343. doi: 10.1257/pol.20140283
Sohn, S. H., Joo, S. H., Grable, J. E., Lee, S., and Kim, M. (2012). Adolescents' financial literacy: the role of financial socialization agents, financial experiences, and money attitudes in shaping financial literacy among South Korean youth. J. Adolesc. 35, 969–980. doi: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2012.02.002
Sohn, S. H., Son, S., and Seo, W. (2022). Effects of an e-mentoring program to improve youth financial empowerment. Hum. Ecol. Res. 60, 549–564. doi: 10.6115/fer.2022.037
Suhana, P. L., Maryanti, R., and Wulandary, V. (2022). Building motivation in saving on elementary school students through financial literacy. Indonesian J. Multidic. Res. 2, 131–136. doi: 10.17509/ijomr.v2i1.38657
Suri, A. K., and Purohit, S. (2017). An analysis of personal financial literacy among expatriates in the United Arab Emirates. Contemp. Rev. Middle East 4, 278–296. doi: 10.1177/2347798917711073
Suseno, I., Yohanna, L., and Sondari, E. (2021). “Islamic financial literacy and management for msmes,” in Proceeding of the International Conference on Family Business and Entrepreneurship , 2. doi: 10.33021/icfbe.v2i1.3540
Sutter, M., Zoller, C., and Glätzle-Rützler, D. (2019). Economic behavior of children and adolescents–A first survey of experimental economics results. Eur. Econ. Rev. 111, 98–121. doi: 10.1016/j.euroecorev.2018.09.004
Swiecka, B., Yeşildag, E., Özen, E., and Grima, S. (2020). Financial literacy: the case of Poland. Sustainability 12:700. doi: 10.3390/su12020700
Tang, N., and Baker, A. (2016). Self-esteem, financial knowledge and financial behavior. J. Econ. Psychol. 54, 164–176. doi: 10.1016/j.joep.2016.04.005
Tavares, F. O., and Santos, E. (2020). Financial literacy perception scale for the Portuguese population. Sci. Ann. Econ. Bus. 67, 277–290. doi: 10.47743/saeb-2020-0015
Thorp, S., Liu, J., Agnew, J., Bateman, H., Eckert, C., and Iskhakov, F. (2023). Feeling comfortable with a mortgage: the impact of framing, financial literacy and advice. J. Finan. Liter. Wellbeing 1, 47–78. doi: 10.1017/flw.2023.3
Tran, T. V. (2022). Impact of financial literacy on vietnamese students' spending management. VNU J. Econ. Bus. 2:4800. doi: 10.57110/jeb.v2i4.4800
Ullah, S., and Yusheng, K. (2020). Financial socialization, childhood experiences and financial well-being: the mediating role of locus of control. Front. Psychol. 11:2162. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.02162
Van Campenhout, G. (2015). Revaluing the role of parents as financial socialization agents in youth financial literacy programs. J. Cons. Affairs 49, 186–222. doi: 10.1111/joca.12064
Venkatesan, T., and Venkataraman, R. (2018). Analysis of factors determining financial literacy using structural equation modelling. SDMIMD J. Manag. 2018, 19–29. doi: 10.18311/sdmimd/2018/19998
Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in Society: The Development of Higher Psychological Processes. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.
Watanapongvanich, S., Binnagan, P., Putthinun, P., Khan, M. S. R., and Kadoya, Y. (2021). Financial literacy and gambling behavior: evidence from Japan. J. Gambl. Stud. 37, 445–465. doi: 10.1007/s10899-020-09936-3
Watanapongvanich, S., Khan, M. S. R., Putthinun, P., Ono, S., and Kadoya, Y. (2022). Financial literacy and gambling behavior in the United States. J. Gambl. Stud. 38, 445–463. doi: 10.1007/s10899-021-10030-5
Wolla, S. (2017). Evaluating the effectiveness of an online module for increasing financial literacy. Soc. Stud. Res. Pract. 12, 154–167. doi: 10.1108/SSRP-04-2017-0014
Yahiaoui, N. E. H. (2023). Demographic and socio-economic determinants of multidimensional financial literacy among young algerian university students. J. Manag. Bus. Educ. 6, 199–221. doi: 10.35564/jmbe.2023.0010
Yanto, H., Ismail, N., Kiswanto, K., Rahim, N. M., and Baroroh, N. (2021). The roles of peers and social media in building financial literacy among the millennial generation: a case of indonesian economics and business students. Cogent Soc. Sci. 7:1947579. doi: 10.1080/23311886.2021.1947579
Ye, J., and Kulathunga, K. (2019). How does financial literacy promote sustainability in smes? A developing country perspective. Sustainability 11:2990. doi: 10.3390/su11102990
Yin, K., Yusof, R., and Abe, Y. (2022). Integrating financial literacy into economics courses through digital tools: the Finlite app. J. Int. Educ. Bus. 15, 331–350. doi: 10.1108/JIEB-06-2021-0068
Yong, H. N. A., and Tan, K. L. (2017). The influence of financial literacy towards risk tolerance. Int. J. Bus. Soc. 18, 469–484. doi: 10.33736/ijbs.3139.2017
Zaleskiewicz, T., and Traczyk, J. (2020). Emotions and financial decision making. Psychol. Perspect. Finan. Dec. Making 9, 107–133. doi: 10.1007/978-3-030-45500-2_6
Zhao, H., and Zhang, L. (2020). Talking money at home: the value of family financial socialization. Int. J. Bank Market. 38, 1617–1634. doi: 10.1108/IJBM-04-2020-0174
Zhou, Y., Yang, M., and Gan, X. (2023). Education and financial literacy: evidence from compulsory schooling law in China. Quart. Rev. Econ. Finance 89, 335–346. doi: 10.1016/j.qref.2022.09.006
Zhu, A. Y. F. (2018). Parental socialization and financial capability among Chinese adolescents in Hong Kong. J. Fam. Econ. Issues 39, 566–576. doi: 10.1007/s10834-018-9584-5
Zhu, A. Y. F. (2019). School financial education and parental financial socialization: findings from a sample of Hong Kong adolescents. Child. Youth Serv. Rev. 107:104532. doi: 10.1016/j.childyouth.2019.104532
Zhu, A. Y. F., and Chou, K. L. (2020). Financial literacy among Hong Kong's Chinese adolescents: testing the validity of a scale and evaluating two conceptual models. Youth Soc. 52, 548–573. doi: 10.1177/0044118X17753813
Zhu, A. Y. F., Yu, C. W. M., and Chou, K. L. (2021). Improving financial literacy in secondary school students: an randomized experiment. Youth Soc. 53, 539–562. doi: 10.1177/0044118X19851311
Keywords: financial education, youth and money, economic autonomy, financial preparedness, educational innovation, family influence, financial technologies, economic behavior
Citation: Mancone S, Tosti B, Corrado S, Spica G, Zanon A and Diotaiuti P (2024) Youth, money, and behavior: the impact of financial literacy programs. Front. Educ. 9:1397060. doi: 10.3389/feduc.2024.1397060
Received: 07 March 2024; Accepted: 07 October 2024; Published: 24 October 2024.
Reviewed by:
Copyright © 2024 Mancone, Tosti, Corrado, Spica, Zanon and Diotaiuti. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY) . The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Pierluigi Diotaiuti, p.diotaiuti@unicas.it
Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
IMAGES
COMMENTS
The inaugural issue of the Journal of Financial Literacy and Wellbeing covers topics that are at the center of academic research, from the effects of financial education in school and the workplace to the importance of financial literacy for the macro-economy. It also covers financial inclusion and how financial literacy can promote the use of ...
The table highlights a few key findings. First, few people across countries can correctly answer three basic financial literacy questions. In the U.S., only 30 percent can do so, with similar low percentages in countries having well-developed financial markets (Germany, the Netherlands, Japan, Australia and others), as well as in nations where financial markets are changing rapidly (Russia and ...
A gender gap in financial literacy is also present across countries. Women are less likely than men to answer questions correctly. ... tasked with designing and implementing the national strategy for financial literacy. I will be able to apply my research to policy and program initiatives in Italy to promote financial literacy: it is an ...
With the need for this research clearly established, the current study formally attempts: 1) To combine the literature at the intersection of financial literacy and financial inclusion through a systematic mapping study and literature review; 2) To study the evolution of financial literacy, and financial inclusion in empirical literature; 3) To ...
This study unveils that financial literacy gives impacts on personal financial decisions, behaviour, saving and retirement, investment, financial risk tolerance, business, and national economy.
PDF | On Dec 1, 2019, Annamaria Lusardi published Financial literacy and the need for financial education: evidence and implications | Find, read and cite all the research you need on ResearchGate
The study also analyses the perception‐reality gap in financial literacy and the attitude of young adults towards financial education. Primary data collected using a structured questionnaire from 736 young adults is used in this study. ... Consequently, research in the area of financial literacy has gained momentum, especially in the ...
1. Introduction. Many existing studies have demonstrated the importance of financial literacy. As a key determinant of wealth inequality, financial knowledge helps individuals make investment decisions and better allocate financial resources (Lusardi et al., Reference Lusardi, Michaud and Mitchell 2017).People with more financial knowledge learn about new financial issues faster (Delavande et ...
This research gap underscores the need for comprehensive studies that evaluate the effectiveness of financial literacy programs targeted at younger populations, exploring variables such as age, socio-economic background, and educational content delivery methods. ... reflecting the diversity of approaches and findings in financial literacy ...
According to Chinese research, financial literacy has a considerable impact on the portfolio diversification of family wealth (Peng et al., 2022). ... If it did not, then it is evident that there is a gap for future research. In terms of purpose and methods are chosen, it is hoped that readers may understand that it is not exhaustive and that ...