Advertisement

Advertisement

Online Gaming Addiction and Basic Psychological Needs Among Adolescents: The Mediating Roles of Meaning in Life and Responsibility

  • Original Article
  • Open access
  • Published: 10 January 2023

Cite this article

You have full access to this open access article

video game addiction research paper

  • Alican Kaya   ORCID: orcid.org/0000-0003-2933-0161 1 ,
  • Nuri Türk   ORCID: orcid.org/0000-0002-7059-9528 2 ,
  • Hasan Batmaz   ORCID: orcid.org/0000-0002-5979-1586 3 &
  • Mark D. Griffiths   ORCID: orcid.org/0000-0001-8880-6524 4  

67k Accesses

8 Citations

3 Altmetric

Explore all metrics

Individuals whose basic needs are naturally satisfied are much less dependent on their environment and more autonomous. Basic psychological needs (i.e., the general motivators of human actions) are significant predictors of online gaming addiction. Moreover, it has been posited that meaning and responsibility in life are at the center of life from an existential point of view. Therefore, a hypothetical model was tested to examine the relationships between basic psychological needs (autonomy, competence, relatedness), online gaming addiction, responsibility, and meaning in life. Data were collected from a sample of 546 participants. Mediation analysis was conducted, and the results indicated that basic psychological needs, online gaming addiction, responsibility, and meaning in life had significant negative and positive relationships. The findings indicated that responsibility and meaning in life had a serial mediating effect in the relationship between basic psychological needs and online gaming addiction. The findings also showed that the inverse relationship between online gaming addiction and basic psychological needs was at least partially explained by meaning in life and responsibility. The results of the present study are of great importance and suggest that interventions to satisfy the basic psychological needs of adolescents may help prevent online gaming addiction.

Similar content being viewed by others

video game addiction research paper

How the Dark Triad associated with internet gaming disorder? The serial mediation of basic psychological needs satisfaction and negative coping styles

video game addiction research paper

The Mechanism Underlying the Effect of Actual-Ideal Self-Discrepancy on Internet Gaming Addiction: a Moderated Mediation Model

video game addiction research paper

Examining the psychometric properties of the electronic gaming motives questionnaire in a sample of Canadian adults: a replication and extension study

Avoid common mistakes on your manuscript.

Introduction

Technological addictions have become an area of increasing research interest and are conceptualized as non-chemical (i.e., behavioral) addictions (Kuss & Billieux, 2017 ). Moreover, they can be engaged in actively or passively (Widyanto & Griffiths, 2006 ). For example, television addiction is a passive technological addiction, whereas smartphone addiction and Internet addiction are active technological addictions (Griffiths, 2017 ). Online addictions have increased rapidly due to the increased use of smartphones, tablets, and laptops. Furthermore, overuse of the Internet has been conceptualized in a number of different ways, including problematic Internet use (Aboujaoude et al., 2006 ; Young, 2009 ), excessive Internet use (Choi et al., 2009 ; Lee et al., 2008 ), and Internet addiction (Griffiths, 2017 ) with some considering it to be an impulsive disorder (Young & Rodgers, 2009 ). In addition, online gaming addiction, which is another addiction associated with the Internet, is defined by the American Psychiatric Association (APA, 2013 ) as the consistent and prolonged use of the Internet to play videogames, frequently with other gamers, that causes disruption and clinically impairs several aspects of a person’s life (e.g., personal relationships, occupation and/or education). Key characteristics of online gaming addiction are individuals obsessively playing online videogames to the point of neglecting everything else in their lives, which leads to social and/or psychological disorders in such individuals (Ates et al., 2018 ; Batmaz & Çelik, 2021 ).

Previous studies have indicated various variables that predict and/or are associated with gaming addiction, including attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD), anxiety and depression (Andreassen et al., 2016 ), social anxiety (Karaca et al., 2020 ), low self-esteem (Kim et al., 2022 ), inter-personal competence (Lee et al., 2019 ), relationship problems and relationship problems, and hostile family environment (Sela et al., 2020 ). In addition, social skill deficits (Mun & Lee, 2022 ), social and psychological isolation (Young, 2009 ), perceived stress (Rajab et al., 2020 ), suicidality (Erevik et al., 2022 ), and aggressive behaviors (McInroy & Mishna, 2017 ) have been reported among individuals who develop gaming addiction.

Although online gaming meets the various needs of individuals, when the behavior turns into an addiction, it leads to adverse effects on individuals, especially adolescents, where it can impair their mental health (Batmaz et al., 2020 ; Purwaningsih & Nurmala, 2021 ). Among adolescents, online gaming addiction has been reported to disrupt mental health, increase depression, anxiety, and psychoticism, disrupt family relationships (De Pasquale et al., 2020 ), lower quality of life (Beranuy et al., 2020 ), increase social phobia (Wei et al., 2012 ), lower school performance, and improve sleep deprivation (Chamarro et al., 2020 ; Király et al., 2015 ). In short, online gaming addiction negatively affects adolescents’ lives in different areas (Griffiths, 2022 ; Haberlin & Atkin, 2022 ). Therefore, research is needed to delineate the causes of online gaming addiction, eliminate its adverse effects, and implement necessary treatment.

Although many studies have been conducted examining online game addiction among adolescents (see Rosendo-Rios et al., 2022 ) for a recent review of studies), there are few studies examining the relationship between basic psychological needs and online game addiction (Bekir & Celik, 2019 ). In the present study, it is posited that basic psychological needs could be predictors due to the relationship with gaming disorders and problematic gaming (Allen & Anderson, 2018 ; Liu et al., 2021 ; Yu et al., 2015 ). When basic psychological needs are not met, it pushes individuals to exhibit maladaptive behavioral reactions (i.e., online gaming addiction) (Bekir & Çelik, 2019 ). In addition, few studies have addressed the relationship between responsibility and meaning in life and online game addiction (Arslan, 2021 ; Kaya, 2021 ). Moreover, no study has ever examined the mediating role of responsibility and meaning in life in the relationship between basic psychological needs and online game addiction. For these reasons, the present study examined the mediating roles of responsibility and meaning in life in explaining the relationship between basic psychological needs and online game addiction.

Online Gaming Addiction and Basic Psychological Needs

Self-determination theory is a well-established motivational theory comprising six mini-theories (Ryan & Deci, 2017 ). One of these mini-theories is the Basic Psychological Needs Theory (BPNT), which claims that the satisfaction of basic psychological needs is associated with better health and greater psychological well-being (Ryan & Deci, 2000 ). Basic psychological needs are requirements for psychological development, integrity, and well-being (Deci & Ryan, 2000 ). In contrast to the often-frustrating real world, videogames are designed to satisfy all three psychological needs (i.e., autonomy, competence, and relatedness) (Rigby & Ryan, 2011 ). Satisfaction of the needs for competence, autonomy, and relatedness can explain large amounts of the variance in game enjoyment (Rigby & Ryan, 2011 ; Tamborini et al., 2011 ). Online gaming can fulfill the (i) need for relatedness by directing players to social relationships with real or fictional characters, (ii) need for autonomy by giving them management and control within the game, and (iii) need for competence by making them feel successful in playing challenging videogames (Allen & Anderson, 2018 ).

Individuals addicted to videogames need novelty seeking, socialization, competition, and/or entertainment (Hussain et al., 2012 ; Larrieu et al., 2022 ). Studies have shown that gaming addiction is related to basic needs (Billieux et al., 2015 ) and psychological needs such as success, independence, fun, and respect (Herodotou et al., 2012 ). The increasing demand for playing videogames shows that adolescents try to satisfy some of their psychological needs via the Internet (Shen et al., 2013 ; Turan, 2021 ). One longitudinal study found that problematic online gaming and satisfaction of basic psychological needs were positively associated (Yu et al., 2015 ). It has also been reported that adolescents whose basic psychological needs were not met and whose perceived social support was low had high levels of gaming addiction (Yıldırım & Zeren, 2021 ). In this context, some studies claim that online games are tools for satisfying basic psychological needs (Oliver et al., 2016 ). However, studies have shown that the low level of basic psychological need satisfaction in real life can be met with high need satisfaction in online gaming, which leads to addiction for a small minority (Kardefelt-Winther, 2014 ; Rigby & Ryan, 2017 ; Wu et al., 2013 ). Based on the aforementioned literature, it was expected that there would be a significant negative relationship between basic psychological needs derived from real-life and online gaming addiction.

Online Gaming Addiction and Meaning in Life

The debate about the meaning in life has been ongoing for years (Yalom, 2020 ). Because there are many definitions of meaning in life, making a standard definition of meaning in life has been difficult (King & Hicks, 2021 ; Park, 2010 ). Meaning in life is a multifaceted construct conceptualized in various ways that address the value and purpose of life, meaningful life goals, and sometimes spirituality (Jim et al., 2006 ). According to Ryff ( 1989 ), meaning in life is a sign of a sense of direction, goals, and well-being. Frankl ( 2009 ) states that meaning in life differs from individual to individual, day to day, and hour to hour. Many studies have been conducted regarding meaning in life and concepts in the literature. For instance, some of these studies assert that meaning in life increases happiness (Debats et al., 1993 ) and life satisfaction (Yıkılmaz & Demir Güdül, 2015 ) and that the presence of meaning in life positively affects psychological health (Bailey & Phillips, 2016 ) and has a high level of meaning that can lower the incidence of depression (Mascaro & Rosen, 2005 ).

Similar to the aforementioned studies, adolescents’ having meaning in life can protect them from problematic behaviors such as substance abuse and eating disorders (Brassai et al., 2011 ; Shek et al., 2019 ). Adolescence is a period of seeking identity (Erikson, 1968 ) and decision-making (Marcia, 1980 ). Steger et al., ( 2006 ) pointed out that adolescents’ experience of seeking meaning in life or having a meaning in life may be determinative for successful identity development. However, considering that questioning the meaning in life results from the search for identity, it could be speculated that adolescents who constantly play online videogames will be far from such a search. Although studies have shown that adolescents search for identity in while online gaming (Monacis et al., 2017 ; Subrahmanyam & Šmahel, 2011 ; Tanhan & Özlem, 2015 ), it has been reported that excessive online gaming can also make this exploration more maladaptive, and this may lead to online gaming addiction (King & Delfabbro, 2014 ; Kokkini et al., 2022 ). One study reported that as gaming addiction decreases among adolescents, the level of meaning in life increases (Kaya, 2021 ). In general, it is expected in the present study that the existence of meaning in life in among adolescents will reduce online gaming addiction.

Online Gaming Addiction and Responsibility

One of the characteristic features of online gaming addiction is that individuals spend their time playing online games by procrastinating and/or not doing their daily work (Thatcheret al., 2008 ). According to the fifth edition of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-5) criteria, one criterion for Internet gaming disorder is that individuals continue to play online games despite being aware of psychosocial problems (American Psychiatric Association, 2013 ). Here, individuals fail to engage in important day-to-day responsibilities and play online games instead. Similarly, it has been shown that online gaming addicts jeopardize or lose their job, education, and/or career opportunities to play online games (Kardefelt-Winther, 2014 ). Time spent playing games instead of engaging in life’s more important tasks can be viewed as a lack of responsibility by individuals themselves, their families, and/or friends (Wartberg et al., 2017 ; Zhang et al., 2019 ).

Responsibility consists of three elements: accountability, liability, and imputability (Robinson, 2009 ). Imputability refers to individuals being responsible for their actions and decisions, accountability refers to fulfilling contractual expectations, and liability refers to assuming a moral responsibility without a contract (Holdorf & Greenwald, 2018 ). The concept of responsibility therapy is defined as the ability of individuals to meet their own needs while allowing others around them to meet their needs (Corey, 2015 ). Being conscious of responsibility means that individuals are aware of themselves and their feelings, thoughts, and pain (Yalom, 2020 ). Dökmen ( 2019 ) defines it as a responsibility to accept the consequences on others of what an individual does or does not do based on his thoughts.

In addition, it is discussed in the literature under two dimensions: emotion (Berkowitz & Daniels, 1963 ; Özen, 2013 ) and behavior (Glasser, 2005 ; Taylı, 2006 ). Individuals with a sense of responsibility have characteristics such as acting with awareness of their own and others’ rights, respecting others, and attempting to fulfill their responsibilities (Özen, 2011 ; Yough et al., 2022 ). On the other hand, individuals who do not have a sense of responsibility make themselves and others feel worthless while living without a plan or program (Cüceloğlu, 2015 ). Studies have shown that a low sense of responsibility can lead to aggression, lying, and avoidance of responsibility, while a high level of responsibility can trigger perfectionism, leading to anxiety, depression, and obsessive-compulsive disorders (Taylı, 2013 ; Wang et al., 2022 ).

The behavior of responsibility, the second sub-dimension of responsibility (Yalom, 2020 ), means that individuals can take responsibility by bearing the consequences of their behavior without attributing it to someone else (Douglass, 2001 ; Shahzadi et al., 2022 ). It has a function that improves positive activities and prevents harmful activities (Kesici, 2018 ). For example, individuals who act responsibly are respected by society and avoid punishment (Douglass, 2001 ). On the other hand, during adolescence, when serious responsibilities begin to be undertaken, a minority of individuals may move away from social life due to gaming addiction. Because of this situation, other people in the individual’s social life (e.g., family and friends) become unimportant to adolescents with low awareness of responsibility. Recent studies have observed that adolescents who excessively play videogames have difficulty fulfilling their responsibilities (Dinçer & Kolan, 2020 ; Doğan & Pamuk, 2022 ). In the present study, it was expected that adolescents with higher levels of responsibility would be less addicted to online gaming (i.e., an inverse relationship).

Basic Psychological Needs, Meaning in Life, Responsibility, and Online Game Addiction

Basic Psychological Needs Theory (BPNT) focuses on the satisfaction and frustration of psychological needs and argues that these needs significantly impact individuals’ psychological health and well-being (Ryan & Deci, 2000 ). Lack of fulfillment of basic psychological needs leads to negative consequences (e.g., depression, stress, and addiction) (Cantarero et al., 2021 ; Levine et al., 2022 ; Orkibi & Ronen, 2017 ; Xiao & Zheng, 2022 ). However, satisfying these needs is associated with positive outcomes such as general self-efficacy (İhsan et al., 2011 ), mental resilience (Kilinç & Gürer, 2019 ), subjective well-being (Akbağ & Ümmet, 2018 ), and obtaining meaning in life (Çelik & Gazioğlu, 2017 ). Furthermore, Weinstein et al. ( 2012 ) suggested that the search for meaning increased significantly when these needs were satisfied. Individuals whose needs are fulfilled are more prone to seek meaning in their life and, therefore, to experience meaning in their life, whereas individuals whose needs are not fulfilled experience a sense of meaninglessness (Eakman, 2013 ). According to Steger ( 2006 ), although individuals continue to search for meaning in one area of their lives, they may have meaning in a different area of their life. Meaning in life is defined as the purpose and importance of the life that individuals derive from their experiences (Baumeister & Vohs, 2002 ; Steger et al., 2006 ). Frankl ( 1969 ) posited that to achieve the meaning of life, an individual must take responsibility for realizing their potential, even at a young age. Therefore, a meaningful life requires individuals taking responsibility for themselves and others.

Responsibility refers to the individual’s sense of duty toward family, friends, and society (Geçtan, 2006 ), and can be examined in personal and social dimensions (Arslan & Wong, 2022 ). Personal responsibility means that an individual is accountable to themselves and to the needs or well-being of others (Ruyter, 2002 ). It also emphasizes self-responsibility by representing the individual’s behaviors and choices that can affect themselves and others (Mergler & Shield, 2016 ). Social responsibility relates to values that support individuals’ moral and prosocial behavior (Wray-Lake & Syvertsen, 2011 ). It includes decisions and actions that benefit others and society (Martins et al., 2015 ). Moreover, it is an important source of support in strengthening individuals’ mental health and improving their life skills (Martins et al., 2017 ) as well as coping with addictions (Amini et al., 2020 ). Therefore, individuals’ personal and social responsibility can protect them against negative situations such as developing addictions (e.g., online gaming addiction) (Chiou & Wan, 2007 ).

Online games allow individuals to meet other players, have fun, achieve status, and obtain financial benefits (Ballabio et al., 2017 ; Columb et al., 2022 ). In addition, escaping from the problems of real life, even temporarily, and achieving relaxation are among the benefits that individuals gain through gaming (Yee, 2006 ). Consequently, online gaming can lead individuals to play online games frequently and for long periods of time, which in turn can lead to the risk of addiction (Luciana, 2010 ; Sachdeva & Verma, 2015 ). The 11th revision of the International Classification of Diseases (ICD-11) characterized gaming disorder as a repetitive or persistent pattern of gaming behavior (World Health Organization, 2019 ). Individuals that are affected by online gaming addiction have also been reported to experience problems with interpersonal relationships (Wongpakaran et al., 2021 ), occupation (Lelonek-Kuleta et al., 2021 ), and health (Chan et al., 2022 ). As such, online gaming addiction can lead to situations that threaten the lives and functionality of individuals through the process and its consequences.

The Present Study

The present study was framed according to self-determination and existentialist positive psychology theories. Self-determination theory (SDT) suggests that the non-satisfaction or inhibition of basic psychological needs can lead to negative consequences (i.e., online gaming addiction). In addition, it emphasizes that behaviors emerge from the individual’s beliefs, meaning, and value judgments rather than external factors (i.e., social norms and group pressure). According to the SDT, need (autonomy, competence, and relatedness) predicts meaning in life (Eakman, 2013 ). Moreover, in a longitudinal study based on SDT, individuals whose basic psychological needs were fulfilled had increased meaning in life (Zhang et al., 2022 ). In addition, the existentialist theory of positive psychology suggests that the meaning in life, which individuals create themselves, can be sustained through responsibility. Individuals having responsibility can also enable them to lead a meaningful life (Arslan & Yıldırım, 2021 ; Wong, 2019 ). According to Wong ( 2010 ), meaning consists of the components of purpose, understanding, responsibility, and enjoyment (PURE). In addition to responsibility being one of the basic concepts that constitute meaning, the search for meaning in life continues intensely during adolescence (Steger, 2012 ). This is especially the case for adolescents who begin to question people and the world deeply, having a meaningful life can protect them from behavioral addictions (Qiu et al., 2022 ; Zhao et al., 2020 ). Considering the role of responsibility and meaning in the life of adolescents, it is important to examine online game addiction, which may be affected by basic psychological needs. Therefore, a serial mediation model was determined based on the assumptions of self-determination and existential positive psychotherapy theory.

In addition to the aforementioned theoretical framework, studies have shown that unfulfilled basic psychological needs are predictors of online gaming addiction (Allen & Anderson, 2018 ; Liang et al., 2021 ; Mills & Allen, 2020 ; Yu et al., 2015 ). However, studies conducted with adolescents have found a relationship between online gaming addiction and responsibility and meaning in life (Doğan & Pamuk, 2022 ; Kaya, 2021 ). In the present study, which also considers the different dynamics in online gaming addiction, a new model is proposed to examine the relationship between basic psychological needs and online gaming addiction through responsibility and meaning in life. In this context, the present study assessed whether basic psychological needs (i.e., autonomy, relatedness, competence) affect the relationship between online gaming addiction, meaning in life, and responsibility among adolescents. Four research questions were investigated: Do basic psychological needs predict online gaming addiction? (RQ1); Does the level of responsibility have a mediating effect on the relationship between basic psychological needs and online game addiction? (RQ2); Does meaning in life have a mediating effect on the relationship between basic psychological needs and online game addiction? (RQ3); Do responsibility and meaning in life have a serial mediating effect on the relationship between basic psychological needs and online game addiction? (RQ4).

Participants

Power analysis was performed via the G* Power 3.1.9.7 program to determine the sample size required for the present study. For this purpose, at the conventional significance level of 0.05 and power at 0.80, a small effect size is determined as r = 0.20 (Cohen, 2013 ). As a result of the analysis, it was determined that the required sample size was 395. The sample in the present study comprised 546 individuals (393 females and 153 males). The participants ranged from 15 to 18 years old, with a mean age of 16.25 years (SD ± 0.82). Just below half the sample of the participants were in the 9th grade ( n =252; 46.2%), 156 were in the 10th grade (28.6%), 74 were in the 11th grade (13.6%), and 64 were in the 12th grade (11.7%). Over one-third of the sample self-reported their socioeconomic status (SES) as being low ( n =210; 38.5%), 224 reported it as being medium (41.0%), and 112 reported it as being high (20.5%). Participants stated that they played videogames 3.56 h daily on average (SD ± 3.12). The number of devices they used to play online videogames was 2.09 (SD ± 0.96).

Basic Psychological Needs Scale (BPNS)

The 21-item BPNS (Deci & Ryan, 2000 ; Turkish version: Kesici et al., 2003 ) was used to assess basic psychological needs. The scale consists of three subscales: (i) autonomy (AU), (ii) competence (CMP), and (iii) relatedness (RLT). The scale has 21 items that tap into the satisfaction of autonomy (e.g., “I feel free to decide how to live my life”), relatedness (e.g., “There aren’t many people in my life that I feel close to”), and competence (e.g., “The people I know say that I am successful in what I do”) which are rated on five-point Likert scale from 1 ( strongly disagree ) to 5 ( strongly agree ). The higher the score, the greater fulfillment of autonomy, competence, and relatedness. In the present study, the scale’s internal reliabilities for the need for autonomy were α=.76, McDonald’s ω= 76; the need for competence were α =.67, McDonald’s ω= 68; and the need for relatedness were α =.82, McDonald’s ω= 83.

Meaning in Life Questionnaire Scale (MILQS)

The 10-item MILQS (Steger et al., 2006 ; Turkish version: Demirbaş-Çelik and İşmen-Gazioğlu, 2015 ) was used to assess meaning in life. Items (e.g., “I’m always looking for my life’s purpose”) are rated on seven-point Likert scale from 1 ( definitely disagree ) to 7 ( definitely agree ). The total score ranges between 10 and 70. The higher the score, the higher the individual’s level of search for meaning in life. In the present study, the internal reliability for the existence of meaning in life was α=.85 and for seeking meaning in life was α=.82. For the overall scale, Cronbach’s α was .67, and McDonald’s ω was .72.

Sense of Responsibility and Behavior Scale (SRBS)

The 18-item SRBS (Özen, 2013 ) was used to assess responsibility. Items (e.g., “I feel responsible for being a member of charitable organizations”) are rated on four-point scale ranging from 1 ( never ) to 4 ( always ). The total score ranges between 18 and 72. The higher the score, the greater the level of responsibility. The SRBS consists of two subscales and each can be used separately. The sense of responsibility sub-dimension was used in the present study. For this sub-dimension, Cronbach’s α was .86, and McDonald’s ω was .87.

Online Game Addiction Scale (OGAS)

The 21-item OGAS (Başol & Kaya, 2018 ) was used to assess online gaming addiction. Items (e.g., “My friendships were damaged/broken due to online games”) are rated on a five-point Likert scale ranging from 1 ( absolutely disagree ) to 5 ( absolutely agree ). The total score ranges between 21 and 105 points. The higher the score, the greater the risk of online gaming addiction. In the present study, Cronbach’s α was .88, and McDonald’s ω was .89.

Procedure and Ethics

Participants were selected from three different high schools in Turkey in the cities of Ağrı, Karabük, and Siirt. The schools were informed about the purpose and duration of the study. The researchers visited the schools, and informed consent forms were distributed. Written informed consent forms were obtained from the legal guardians or parents of the adolescents who volunteered to participate in the study. The purpose of the study was explained to the participants. The eligibility criteria for participation in the study were being an adolescent and being an individual who played (or used to play) one or more online videogames. An online link to the survey was sent to the participants, and each participant was allowed to complete the survey only once. All data were collected using Google Forms in the classroom. Participants were reminded that they might stop answering at any stage of the survey process if they wanted to. Participants were asked not to provide personal information to ensure anonymity and confidentiality. Ethics committee approval of this research was obtained from Ağrı İbrahim Çeçen University (reference number: 110), and every research stage was carried out in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki.

Data Analysis

All analyses were carried out using SPSS version 26, Hayes’ ( 2018 ) PROCESS Macro (version 3), and G* Power 3.1.9.7 programs. Before starting the analysis, the necessary assumptions to perform the analysis were tested. The kurtosis and skewness values were examined to understand whether the assumptions required for the prerequisites of parametric tests were met. The skewness and kurtosis values for a normal distribution have acceptable threshold values if they are ±2 (George, 2010 ). There were no assumption violations in the research data. In addition, it was found that the correlation between the study variables was not high. The correlations ranged between .17 and .63 ( p <.001). The research variables were also examined to ensure there were no multicollinearity issues. When the tolerance, variance inflation factor (VIF), and confidence interval (CI) values were examined, these values were all within acceptable limits. It was determined that VIF was between 1.12 and 1.48, the tolerance value was between .67 and .89, and CI was between 7.21 and 17.88. The limit values required to avoid multicollinearity problems are more than 0.20 for the tolerance value, less than 10 for the VIF value, and less than 30 for the CI value (Albayrak, 2005 ; Büyüköztürk, 2016 ; Şata, 2020 ). Consequently, no multicollinearity problems were detected. Mahalanobis distance values were examined to determine whether there were outliers in the sample. A total of 21 outliers were identified in the dataset. These outliers were excluded from the analysis, meaning the final sample size was 546. SPSS PROCESS macro was utilized to conduct mediation analyses (Hayes, 2018 ). The bootstrapping method was employed with 5000 resampling and 95% confidence intervals (CIs) to test the significance of the mediating pathways. An effect is deemed significant if the confidence interval does not contain zero (Preacher & Hayes, 2008 ).

Table 1 shows the correlations between all the main variables in the study (basic psychological needs (autonomy, competence, relatedness), online gaming addiction, responsibility, and meaning in life). Pearson correlations indicated that all variables were significantly (albeit moderately and weakly) related.

Serial Multiple Mediational Analyses—Modeling Data

Table 2 , Table 3 , and Table 4 show the results of the serial mediation analysis. First, there was a direct effect of autonomy on online gaming addiction ( β =−.67, p <.001). Moreover, the relationship between competence and online gaming addiction was examined. There was a direct effect of competence on online gaming addiction ( β =−.63, p < . 001). When the relationship between relatedness, the last of the basic psychological needs, and online gaming addiction was examined, there was a direct effect of relatedness on online gaming addiction ( β =−.48, p < . 001). There was also a significant indirect effect of autonomy on online gaming addiction via responsibility (indirect effect=−.12, SE=.02, 95% CI= [−.20, −.06]). Also, the indirect effect of competence on online gaming addiction via responsibility was significant (indirect effect=−.19, SE=.02, 95% CI= [−.31, −.10]). Lastly, the indirect effect of relatedness on online gaming addiction via responsibility was significant (indirect effect=−.17, SE=.01, 95% CI= [−.26, −.10]).

When indirect effects were examined, there was a significant indirect effect of autonomy on online gaming addiction via meaning in life (indirect effect=−.07, SE=.02, 95% CI= [−.14, −.00]). Also, the indirect effect of competence on online gaming addiction via meaning in life was significant (indirect effect=−.11, SE=.02, 95% CI= [−.22, −.00]). Lastly, the indirect effect of relatedness on online gaming addiction via meaning in life was significant (indirect effect=−.05, SE=.01, 95% CI= [−.10, −.01]).

Moreover, the indirect effects of autonomy on online gaming addiction via meaning in life and responsibility were tested. The effect was significant (testing serial multiple mediation; effect=−.04 SE=.01, 95% CI= [−.07, −.01]). Also, the indirect effects of competence on online gaming addiction via meaning in life and responsibility were tested. The effect was significant (testing serial multiple mediation; effect=−.02 SE=.01, 95% CI= [−.04, −.00]). Moreover, the indirect effects of relatedness on online gaming addiction via meaning in life and responsibility were tested. The effect was significant (testing serial multiple mediation; effect=−.02 SE=.01, 95% CI= [−.06, −.01]). In the relationship between basic psychological needs (autonomy, competence, relatedness) and online gaming addiction, meaning in life and responsibility had serial mediating effects.

The results indicated that autonomy predicted online gaming addiction. When autonomy was entered as the predictor, it significantly predicted online gaming addiction ( β  = −0.67, t = −5.58,  p  < .001), and accounted for 5.4% of the variance in the model. Figure 1  shows the regression coefficients of the mediation model. The indirect path mediated by responsibility ( β =−.12, 95% CI= [−.20, −.06]) produced a higher change in variance than the indirect path mediated by meaning in life ( β =−.07, 95% CI= [−.14, −.00]) in the relationship between relatedness and online gaming addiction (see Table 2 ). Therefore, responsibility appeared to have a higher effect than meaning in life. Autonomy predicted a higher level of meaning in life. It also predicted a higher level of responsibility. Higher meaning in life was associated with a higher level of responsibility. Higher level of responsibility was associated with lower online gaming addiction. Consequently, the results indicated that the relationship between autonomy and online gaming addiction was partially mediated by meaning in life and responsibility (see Fig. 1 ).

figure 1

The results of the serial multiple mediational models

It was also found that competence predicted online gaming addiction. There was also an indirect relationship between competence and online gaming addiction ( β = −0.64, t = −4.13, p < .001), accounting for 4.7% of the variance in the model. Competence predicted meaning and responsibility in life. The indirect path mediated by responsibility ( β =−.19, 95% CI= [−.31, −.10]) produced a higher change in variance than the indirect path mediated by meaning in life ( β =−.11, 95% CI= [−.22, −.00]) in the relationship between competence and online gaming addiction Furthermore, the relationship between competence and online gaming addiction was mediated by meaning in life and responsibility separately (see Table 3 ). The results also showed that meaning in life and responsibility had serial mediation effects in the relationship between competence and online gaming addiction (see Fig. 2 ).

figure 2

Lastly, the results indicated that relatedness predicted online gaming addiction ( β = −0.48, t = −4.63, p < .001). When relatedness was included in the model, it was found that it accounted for 3.8% of the variance. Moreover, there was also an indirect relationship between relatedness and online gaming addiction. When the indirect effects are examined, the indirect path mediated by responsibility ( β =−.17, 95% CI= [−.26, −.10]) produced a higher change in variance than the indirect path mediated by meaning in life ( β =−.05, 95% CI= [−.10, −.01]) in the relationship between relatedness and online gaming addiction (see Table 4 ). The results suggested that the relationship between relatedness and online gaming addiction was partially mediated by meaning in life and responsibility (see Fig. 3 ).

figure 3

In self-determination theory (SDT), basic psychological needs comprise autonomy, competence, and relatedness. In SDT (Deci & Ryan, 2000 ), basic psychological needs are expressed as essential psychological nutrients for psychological development, integrity, and well-being. Negative psychological consequences occur when requirements are not met, neglected, or prevented (Deci & Ryan, 2000 ). If individuals cannot satisfy a basic need, they engage in activities that give pleasure to individuals momentarily, even if they do not satisfy them (Antunes et al., 2020 ; Deci & Ryan, 2011 ). One of these activities is online gaming, which has an incredibly interactive structure. At the same time, online videogames are appreciated because they create an environment where both the need for relatedness and autonomy are met in the virtual world. Individuals naturally seek new challenges to experience a sense of efficacy even when no external rewards (e.g., money) are earned (Dindar, 2018 ; Matsumoto, 2009 ). The fact that online games have a reward mechanism is suitable for activating feelings of competence among individuals. It is thought that adolescents tend to meet their basic psychological needs (need for autonomy, competence, and relatedness) that they cannot fully meet from their parents or close friends through online gaming.

The present study examined the mediating role of meaning in life and the level of responsibility in the relationship between online gaming addiction and basic psychological needs (i.e., autonomy, competence, and relatedness) among adolescents. Findings showed that autonomy predicted online game addiction. In other words, autonomy had significant negative effect on online game addiction. Considering that addiction is related to reduced autonomy (Amatem, 2008 ), it can be said that the finding is compatible with the literature. However, there is a study in which there was a negative relationship between the need for autonomy and digital game addiction among adolescents (Dursun and Çapan, 2018 ), which supports the research finding. On the contrary, there is a study in which autonomy and online game addiction had significant positive relationships (Bekir and Çelik, 2019 ). Similarly, it is known that the need for autonomy has a positive relationship with social media addiction (Young-Ju et al., 2018 ) and a negative relationship with Internet addiction (Piri et al., 2018 ; Zeren & Can, 2019 ). These studies, which have obtained different results, make the relationship between the need for autonomy and digital addictions open to discussion but also show that further research is needed.

According to the present study’s findings, it was found that relatedness and competence, as well as autonomy, predicted online gaming addiction. Studies have shown that competence and relatedness have significant relationships with online gaming addiction (Bekir and Çelik, 2019 ; Dursun and Çapan, 2018 ). In addition, research has shown that relatedness has a negative relationship with short-form video addiction (Yang et al., 2022 ), and relatedness dissatisfaction positively correlates with Internet gaming disorder (Hui et al., 2019 ). Moreover, significant negative relationships have been found between competence and smartphone addiction (Gao et al., 2022 ; Sun et al., 2020 ) and Internet addiction (Zeren & Can, 2019 ; Canoğulları, 2014 ). Based on these results concerning technological addictions, it can be said that the literature findings and the results of the present study are compatible.

The tendency of individuals to play online videogames may be to meet their autonomy, competence, and relatedness needs (Ryan et al., 2006 ). In addition, when basic psychological needs are prevented, technological addictions (gaming addiction, smartphone addiction, social network addiction, and Internet addiction) increase (Gugliandolo et al., 2020 ). This may be the compensation for unmet basic psychological needs through addiction (Kuss et al., 2017 ; Mills et al., 2018 ). Therefore, fulfilling basic psychological needs in real life and eliminating the problems that prevent this satisfaction can be a protective factor against online gaming addiction.

Another finding of the present study was that the level of responsibility hads a mediating effect on the relationship between basic psychological needs and online gaming addiction. However, there was a positive and significant relationship between basic psychological needs and responsibility. In contrast, a significant negative relationship was found between responsibility and online gaming addiction. Considering that the components of responsibility (accountability, liability, and imputability) in Robinson’s ( 2009 ) definition appear less important in online environments, it is assumed that adolescents who are addicted to online gaming experience less sense of responsibility. A recent study found that a higher level of responsibility significantly predicted online gaming addiction, whereas a lower level of responsibility negatively affected online gaming addiction (Kesici, 2020 ).

Research conducted by Arslan ( 2021 ) found that secondary school students’ sense of responsibility and behavior had a crucial predictive role in online gaming addiction. Another study reported a significant negative relationship between the students’ videogame addiction and their personal and social responsibility behavior (Dinçer & Kolan, 2020 ). Based on previous studies and the results of the present study, it is thought that increasing the level of responsibility of secondary and high school students would reduce gaming addiction. Adolescents whose level of responsibility increases are also more likely to engage in responsible behavior. This is supported by studies in the literature that physical education and sports play an essential role in helping adolescents acquire responsible behavior (Bayraktar et al., 2016 ; Bugdayci, 2019 ; Tazegül, 2014 ). These studies’ results are considered necessary regarding online gaming addiction because such behavior leads to a sedentary lifestyle (Cómez-Mármol et al., 2017 ).

Findings indicated that meaning in life had a mediating effect on the relationship between basic psychological needs and online gaming addiction. However, there was a positive and significant relationship between basic psychological needs and meaning in life. In contrast, a significant negative relationship between meaning in life and online gaming addiction was found. These findings demonstrate the importance of meaning in life in preventing online gaming addiction among adolescents. A study by Kaya ( 2021 ) on adolescent online gaming addiction found that as the level of online gaming addiction decreased, the level of meaning in life increased. These results suggest that meaning in life affects online gaming addiction as a cause and consequence. Considering that having a meaningful life increases resilience (Batmaz et al., 2021 ; Doğrusever et al., 2022 ), low resilience increases gaming addiction (Canale et al., 2019 ), and gaming addiction reduces happiness (Kaya, 2021 ; Turan, 2021 ), meaning in life seems to be an essential variable that can affect gaming addiction.

What makes the present study unique to the online gaming addiction literature is that responsibility and meaning in life had a serial mediating effect on the relationship between basic psychological needs and online gaming addiction. In other words, the results indicated that the relationship between relatedness, competence, and autonomy with online gaming addiction was partially mediated by meaning in life and responsibility. This finding suggests that the need for autonomy, competence, and relatedness increases the level of meaning in life, which in turn reduces online game addiction. Similarly, online game addiction can decrease as the need for autonomy, competence, and relatedness increases the level of responsibility. In addition, based on the serial mediation effect, it suggests that meeting the need for autonomy, competence, and relatedness can reduce adolescents’ online game addiction by increasing their meaning in life and their level of responsibility.

To the best of the authors’ knowledge, the present study is the first to examine the mediating role of responsibility and meaning in life between basic psychological needs and online gaming addiction. The associations between these variables provide greater understanding and knowledge concerning online gaming addiction and provide additional insight into the significant causes that underlie playing games online (which may be potential factors in the acquisition, development, and maintenance of online gaming addiction among adolescents). Moreover, fulfilling basic psychological needs appears to increase responsibility and meaning in life and reduce susceptibility to online gaming addiction. The findings enrich the literature because it suggests new protective factors that might prevent adolescents from developing online gaming addiction.

The findings offer relevant practical implications for adolescents, educators, families, private and public health institutions, and mental health professionals to assist them in designing addiction prevention strategies and policies. Results also suggest that basic psychological need satisfaction fulfilment in real life plays an important role in the development and maintenance of online gaming addiction among adolescents. Educators, parents, and adolescents could utilize awareness of the factors contributing to online gaming addiction to help them take preventive measures against it. In addition, if adolescents have high levels of responsibility and meaning in life, it may help reduce online game addiction. Considering the findings, it is recommended that mental health professionals provide training and services that increase the level of responsibility among adolescents and enable them to have meaning in their lives to prevent the onset of online gaming addiction. In addition, private and public health institutions should implement training programs to improve the skills of parents, such as digital parenting, to cope with online gaming addiction. This training should also ensure that parents behave with awareness of the basic psychological needs of adolescents in the family and that they gain thoughts and approaches that can add responsibility and meaning in life.

Limitations

As in all studies, the present study also has some limitations. The first is that the study was cross-sectional. Conducting a cross-sectional study means that causality between the study variables cannot be determined. Second, completing the survey online may have influenced respondents’ responses (with those without home Internet access unable to participate). The online data were also collected during the COVID-19 pandemic. Therefore, adolescents living in isolated environments may have increased their gaming during this period. This unusual situation may have resulted in a lower sense of responsibility and a less meaningful life. This is consistent with the present study’s findings. Another limitation is that the participants were high school students studying in different schools in Turkish provinces, so the findings are not necessarily generalizable to all Turkish schoolchildren. The sample was also limited because it did not include other education levels, such as primary and secondary schools and children from different geographical and cultural regions in Turkey and/or other countries. Future studies are needed with different age groups, such as primary school, secondary school, university students, adults, and various geographical regions in the sample groups (both in and outside Turkey). Such studies are needed to confirm the findings reported here and should include other research designs (e.g., longitudinal studies to determine causality between variables) and other types of data (e.g., qualitative interview data to attain richer data). Another limitation of the present study was that the participant’s responses were self-report and therefore subject to well-established method biases (e.g., social desirability, memory recall).

The study’s findings indicated that adolescents whose basic psychological needs were met exhibited lower levels of online gaming addiction than adolescents whose basic psychological needs were not met. Consequently, the adverse effects of online gaming addiction may be reduced by interventions that meet adolescents’ basic psychological needs. Similarly, a significant negative relationship was found between responsibility and online gaming addiction. Consequently, it appears that adolescents who fulfill the requirements of individual and social responsibilities (studying, spending time with family, going out with friends, etc.) have greater protection from the more negative effects of online gaming. However, when meaning in life and responsibility are included in the relationship between basic psychological needs and online gaming addiction, the effect of basic psychological needs on online game addiction decreases. This suggests that meaning in life and responsibility have a serial mediating role between basic psychological needs and online gaming addiction.

Data Availability

The data that support the findings of this study are available from the first author upon reasonable request.

Aboujaoude, E., Koran, L. M., Gamel, N., Large, M. D., & Serpe, R. T. (2006). Potential markers for problematic internet use: A telephone survey of 2.513 adults. CNS Spectrum, 11 (10), 750–755. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1092852900014875

Article   Google Scholar  

Akbağ, M., & Ümmet, D. (2018). Ana-babaya bağlanma ile öznel iyi oluş arasındaki ilişkide temel psikolojik ihtiyaçların doyumunun aracı rolü.  Turkish Psychological Counseling and Guidance Journal ,  8 (50), 59–85. https://dergipark.org.tr/tr/download/article-file/571505

Albayrak, A. S. (2005). Çoklu doğrusal bağlantı halinde en küçük kareler tekniğinin alternatifi yanlı tahmin teknikleri ve bir uygulama. ZKÜ Sosyal Bilimler Dergisi, 1 (1), 105–126. https://dergipark.org.tr/tr/pub/ijmeb/issue/54840/750869

Allen, J. J., & Anderson, C. A. (2018). Satisfaction and frustration of basic psychological needs in the real world and video games predict internet gaming disorder scores and well being. Computers in Human Behavior, 84 , 220–229. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2018.02.034

Amatem. (2008). Bilgisayar ve internet . Ankara: Amatem Yayınları.

Google Scholar  

American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (DSM-5®) . American Psychiatric Publishing.

Book   Google Scholar  

Amini, Z., & Heidary, B. S. (2020). What components of adolescents’ responsibility are effective in preventing addiction? Advanced Biomedical Research, 9 (1), 2–8. https://doi.org/10.4103/abr.abr_204_19

Andreassen, C. S., Billieux, J., Griffiths, M. D., Kuss, D. J., Demetrovics, Z., Mazzoni, E., & Pallesen, S. (2016). The relationship between addictive use of social media and video games and symptoms of psychiatric disorders: A large-scale cross-sectional study. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 30 (2), 252–262. https://doi.org/10.1037/adb0000160

Antunes, R., Frontini, R., Amaro, N., Salvador, R., Matos, R., Morouço, P., & Rebelo- Gonçalves, R. (2020). Exploring lifestyle habits, physical activity, anxiety and basic psychological needs in a sample of Portuguese adults during COVID-19. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 17 (12), 4360–4372. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17124360

Article   CAS   Google Scholar  

Arslan, A. (2021). Ortaokul öğrencilerinde oyun bağımlılığı ile sorumluluk duygusu davranışı ve sosyal beceriler arasındaki ilişkinin incelenmesi [Unpublished master’s thesis] . İstanbul Sabahattin Zaim Üniversitesi.

Arslan, G., & Wong, P. T. (2022). Measuring personal and social responsibility: an existential positive psychology approach. Journal of Happiness and Health, 2 (1), 1–11. https://doi.org/10.47602/johah.v2i1.5

Arslan, G., & Yıldırım, M. (2021). Perceived risk, positive youth–parent relationships, and internalizing problems in adolescents: Initial development of the Meaningful School Questionnaire. Child Indicators Research, 14 (5), 1911–1929. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12187-021-09841-0

Ates, B., Kaya, A., & Tunç, E. (2018). The ınvestigation of predictors of cyberbullying and cyber victimization in adolescents. International Journal of Progressive Education, 14 (5), 103–118. https://doi.org/10.29329/ijpe.2018.157.9

Bailey, T. H., & Phillips, L. J. (2016). The influence of motivation and adaptation on students’ subjective well-being, meaning in life and academic performance. Higher Education Research & Development, 35 (2), 201–216.

Ballabio, M., Griffiths, M. D., Urbán, R., Quartiroli, A., Demetrovics, Z., & Király, O. (2017). Do gaming motives mediate between psychiatric symptoms and problematic gaming? An empirical survey study. Addiction Research & Theory, 25 (5), 397–408. https://doi.org/10.1080/16066359.2017.1305360

Başol, G., & Kaya, A. B. (2018). Motives and consequences of online game addiction: A scale development study. Archives of Neuropsychiatry, 55 (3), 225–232. https://doi.org/10.5152/npa.2017.17017

Batmaz, H., & Çelik, E. (2021). Examining the online game addiction level in terms of sensation seeking and loneliness in university students. Addicta: The Turkish Journal on Addictions, 8 (2), 126–131. https://doi.org/10.5152/ADDICTA.2021.21017

Batmaz, H., Ulusoy, Y., & İnceoğlu, F. (2020). The mediating role of digital game addiction in the correlation between cyber victimization and cyber bullying. Sciences Studies Journal, 6 (73), 5093–5108. https://doi.org/10.26449/sssj.2726

Batmaz, H., Türk, N., & Doğrusever, C. (2021). The mediating role of hope and loneliness in the relationship between meaningful life and psychological resilience in the Covid-19 Pandemic during. Anemon Muş Alparslan Üniversitesi Sosyal Bilimler Dergisi, 9 (5), 1403–1420. https://doi.org/10.18506/anemon.895199

Baumeister, R. F., & Vohs, K. D. (2002). The pursuit of meaningfulness in life. In: In C. R. Snyder & S. J. Lopez (Eds.), Handbook of Positive Psychology , (pp. 608–618). Oxford University Press

Bayraktar, G., Tozoğlu, E., Gülbahçe, Ö., Öztürk, M. E., & Gülbahçe, A. (2016). Üniversite öğrencilerinin bireysel sosyal sorumluluk düzeylerinin spor ve farklı değişkenler açısından incelenmesi. Uluslararası Hakemli Akademik Spor Sağlık Ve Tıp Bilimleri Dergisi, 18 , 77–88.

Bekir, S., & Çelik, E. (2019). Examining the factors contributing to adolescents’ online game addiction. Anales De Psicología/annals of Psychology, 35 (3), 444–452. https://doi.org/10.6018/analesps.35.3.323681

Berkowitz, L., & Daniels, L. R. (1963). Responsibility and dependency. The Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 66 (5), 429–436. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0049250

Beranuy, M., Machimbarrena, J. M., Asunción Vega-Osés, M., Carbonell, X., Griffiths, M. D., Pontes, H. M., & González-Cabrera, J. (2020). Spanish validation of the Internet Gaming Disorder Scale-Short Form (IGDS9-SF): Prevalence and relationship with online gambling and quality of life. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 17 (5), 1562–1577. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17051562

Billieux, J., Thorens, G., Khazaal, Y., Zullino, D., Achab, S., & Van der Linden, M. (2015). Problematic involvement in online games: A cluster analytic approach. Computers in Human Behavior, 43 , 242–250. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2014.10.055

Brassai, L., Piko, B. F., & Steger, M. F. (2011). Meaning in life: Is it a protective factor for adolescents’ psychological health? International Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 18 (1), 44–51. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12529-010-9089-6

Bugdayci, S. (2019). Examining personal and social responsibility levels of secondary school students. Universal Journal of Educational Research, 7 (1), 206–210. https://doi.org/10.13189/ujer.2019.070126

Büyüköztürk, Ş. (2016). Data analysis handbook for social sciences (22nd ed.). Pegem Akademi Publishing.

Canale, N., Marino, C., Griffiths, M. D., Scacchi, L., Monaci, M. G., & Vieno, A. (2019). The association between problematic online gaming and perceived stress: The moderating effect of psychological resilience. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 8 (1), 174–180. https://doi.org/10.1556/2006.8.2019.01

Canoğulları, Ö. (2014). İnternet bağımlılık düzeyleri farklı ergenlerin cinsiyetlerine göre psikolojik ihtiyaçları, sosyal kaygıları ve anne baba tutum algılarının incelenmesi . (Unpublished Master's thesis), Çukurova Üniversitesi, Adana

Cantarero, K., Van Tilburg, W. A., & Smoktunowicz, E. (2021). Affirming basic psychological needs promotes mental well-being during the COVID-19 outbreak. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 12 (5), 821–828. https://doi.org/10.1177/1948550620942708

Çelik, N. D., & Gazioğlu, E. İ. (2017). Üst-düzey kişilik faktörleri ve yaşamda anlam: temel psikolojik ihtiyaçlarin araci rolü. Balıkesir Üniversitesi Sosyal Bilimler Enstitüsü Dergisi, 20 (38), 11–32. https://doi.org/10.31795/baunsobed.645147

Chamarro, A., Oberst, U., Cladellas, R., & Fuster, H. (2020). Effect of the frustration of psychological needs on addictive behaviors in mobile videogamers—The mediating role of use expectancies and time spent gaming. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 17 (17), 6429. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17176429

Chan, G., Huo, Y., Kelly, S., Leung, J., Tisdale, C., & Gullo, M. (2022). The impact of eSports and online video gaming on lifestyle behaviours in youth: A systematic review. Computers in Human Behavior, 126 , 106974. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2021.106974

Chiou, W. B., & Wan, C. S. (2007). Using cognitive dissonance to induce adolescents’ escaping from the claw of online gaming: The roles of personal responsibility and justification of cost. CyberPsychology & Behavior, 10 (5), 663–670. https://doi.org/10.1089/cpb.2007.9972

Choi, K., Son, H., Park, M., Han, J., Kim, K., Lee, B., & Gwak, H. (2009). Internet overuse and excessive daytime sleepiness in adolescents. Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 63 , 455–462. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1440-1819.2009.01925.x

Cohen, J. (2013). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences . Routledge.

Columb, D., Griffiths, M. D., & O’Gara, C. (2022). Online gaming and gaming disorder: More than just a trivial pursuit. Irish Journal of Psychological Medicine, 39 (1), 1–7. https://doi.org/10.1017/ipm.2019.31

Cómez-Mármol, A., Martínez, B. J., Sánchez, E. D., Valero, A., & González-Víllora, S. (2017). Personal and social responsibility development through sport participation in youth scholars. Journal of Physical Education and Sport, 17 (2), 775–782. https://doi.org/10.7752/jpes.2017.02118

Corey, G. (2015). Theory and practice of counseling and psychotherapy. Nelson Education.

Cüceloğlu, D. (2015). Anlamlı ve coşkulu bir yaşam için savaşçı . Remzi Kitabevi.

De Pasquale, C., Sciacca, F., Martinelli, V., Chiappedi, M., Dinaro, C., & Hichy, Z. (2020). Relationship of internet gaming disorder with psychopathology and social adaptation in Italian young adults. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 17 (21), 8201. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17218201

Debats, D. L., Van der Lubbe, P. M., & Wezeman, F. R. (1993). On the psychometric properties of the Life Regard Index (LRI): A measure of meaningful life: An evaluation in three independent samples based on the Dutch version. Personality and Individual Differences, 14 (2), 337–345. https://doi.org/10.1016/0191-8869(93)90132-M

Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2000). The” what” and” why” of goal pursuits: Human needs and the self-determination of behavior. Psychological Inquiry, 11 (4), 227–268. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15327965PLI1104_01

Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2011). Levels of analysis, regnant causes of behavior and well-being: The role of psychological needs. Psychological Inquiry, 22 (1), 17–22. https://doi.org/10.1080/1047840X.2011.545978

Demirbaş-Çelik, N., & İşmen-Gazioğlu, E. (2015). Meaning in life questionnaire high school form: Turkish validity and reliability. Mehmet Akif Ersoy University Journal of Education Faculty, 33 , 42–60.

Dinçer, B., & Kolan, H. İ. (2020). Ortaokul öğrencilerinin bilgisayar oyun bağımlılığı düzeyleri ile sorumluluk davranışı arasındaki ilişki. Kastamonu Eğitim Dergisi, 28 (6), 2319–2330. https://doi.org/10.24106/kefdergi.833550

Dindar, M. (2018). Do people play MMORPGs for extrinsic or intrinsic rewards? Telematics and Informatics, 35 (7), 1877–1886. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tele.2018.06.001

Doğan, D. A., & Pamuk, İ. (2022). Uzaktan eğitim sürecinde ergenlerin çevrimiçi oyunlara yönelik deneyimleri: Fenomenolojik bir araştırma. International Journal of Social Sciences and Education Research, 8 (1), 71–86. https://doi.org/10.24289/ijsser.1032025

Doğrusever, C., Türk, N., & Batmaz, H. (2022). The mediating role of meaningful life in the relationship between self-esteem and psychological resilience. İnönü Üniversitesi Eğitim Fakültesi Dergisi, 23 (2), 910–928. https://doi.org/10.17679/inuefd.1029866

Dökmen, Ü. (2019). Varolmak gelişmek uzlaşmak , İstanbul, Remzi Kitapevi.

Douglass, N. H. (2001). Saygı ve sorumluluk eğitiminde yeni yaklaşımlar . Nobel Yayınları. Dökmen, Ü. (2019). Varolmak, gelişmek, uzlaşmak . Sistem Yayıncılık

Dursun, A., & Çapan, B. E. (2018). Ergenlerde dijital oyun bağımlılığı ve psikolojik ihtiyaçlar. İnönü Üniversitesi Eğitim Fakültesi Dergisi, 19 (2), 128–140. https://doi.org/10.17679/inuefd.336272

Eakman, A. M. (2013). Relationships between meaningful activity, basic psychological needs, and meaning in life: Test of the meaningful activity and life meaning model. OTJR: Occupation, Participation and Health, 33 (2), 100–109. https://doi.org/10.3928/15394492-20130222-02

Erevik, E. K., Landrø, H., Mattson, Å. L., Kristensen, J. H., Kaur, P., & Pallesen, S. (2022). Problem gaming and suicidality: A systematic literature review. Addictive Behaviors Reports, 15 , 100419. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.abrep.2022.100419

Erikson, E. (1968). Identity, youth and crisis . Norton.

Frankl, V. (1969). The will to meaning . New American Library.

Frankl, V. E. (2009). İnsanın anlam arayışı . Okuyan Us Yayıncılık.

Gao, Q., Zheng, H., Sun, R., & Lu, S. (2022). Parent-adolescent relationships, peer relationships and adolescent mobile phone addiction: The mediating role of psychological needs satisfaction. Addictive Behaviors, 129 , 107260. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2022.107260

Geçtan, E. (2006). İnsan olmak (5. Baskı). Metis

George, D. (2010). SPSS for windows step by step: A simple study guide and reference, 17.0 update (10th ed.). Pearson

Glasser, W. (2005). Responsibility, respect and relationships: Creating emotionally safe classrooms . Quality Educational Programs Inc.

Griffiths, M. D. (2017). Behavioural addiction and substance addiction should be defined by their similarities not their dissimilarities. Addiction, 112 (10), 1718–1720. https://doi.org/10.1111/add.13828

Griffiths, M. D. (2022). Online gaming addiction in youth: Some comments on Rosendo-Rios et al. (2022). Addictive Behaviors, 130 , 107311. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2022.107311

Gugliandolo, M. C., Costa, S., Kuss, D. J., Cuzzocrea, F., & Verrastro, V. (2020). Technological addiction in adolescents: The interplay between parenting and psychological basic needs. International Journal of Mental Health and Addiction, 18 (5), 1389–1402. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11469-019-00156-4

Haberlin, K. A., & Atkin, D. J. (2022). Mobile gaming and Internet addiction: When is playing no longer just fun and games? Computers in Human Behavior, 126 , 106989. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2021.106989

Hayes, A. F. (2018). Partial, conditional, and moderated moderated mediation: Quantification, inference, and interpretation. Communication Monographs, 85 (1), 4–40. https://doi.org/10.1080/03637751.2017.1352100

Herodotou, C., Winters, N., & Kambouri, M. (2012). A motivationally oriented approach to understanding game appropriation. International Journal of Human-Computer Interaction, 28 (1), 34–47. https://doi.org/10.1080/10447318.2011.566108

Holdorf, W. E., & Greenwald, J. M. (2018). Toward a taxonomy and unified construct of responsibility. Personality and Individual Differences, 132 , 115–125. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2018.05.028

Hui, B. P. H., Wu, A. M., Siu, N. Y., Chung, M. L., & Pun, N. (2019). The effects of need satisfaction and dissatisfaction on flourishing among young Chinese gamers: The mediating role of internet gaming disorder. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 16 (22), 4367. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph16224367

Hussain, Z., Griffiths, M. D., & Baguley, T. (2012). Online gaming addiction: Classification, prediction and associated risk factors. Addiction Research & Theory, 20 (5), 359–371. https://doi.org/10.3109/16066359.2011.640442

İhsan, S., Yenigün, Ö., Altıncı, E. E., & Öztürk, A. (2011). Temel psikolojik ihtiyaçların tatmininin genel öz yeterlik ve sürekli kaygı üzerine etkisi (Sakarya Üniversitesi Spor Yöneticiliği Bölümü örneği). Spormetre Beden Eğitimi Ve Spor Bilimleri Dergisi, 9 (4), 149–156. https://doi.org/10.1501/Sporm_0000000212

Jim, H. S., Richardson, S. A., Golden-Kreutz, D. M., & Andersen, B. L. (2006). Strategies used in coping with a cancer diagnosis predict meaning in life for survivors. Health Psychology, 25 , 753–761. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-6133.25.6.753

Karaca, S., Karakoc, A., Can Gurkan, O., Onan, N., & Unsal Barlas, G. (2020). Investigation of the online game addiction level, sociodemographic characteristics and social anxiety as risk factors for online game addiction in middle school students. Community Mental Health Journal, 56 (5), 830–838. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10597-019-00544-z

Kardefelt-Winther, D. (2014). Problematizing excessive online gaming and its psychological predictors. Computers in Human Behavior, 31 , 118–122. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2013.10.017

Kaya, A. (2021). Adölesanlarda dijital oyun bağımlılığının mutluluk ve yaşamın anlamına etkisi. Bağımlılık Dergisi, 22 (3), 297–304. https://doi.org/10.51982/bagimli.902685

Kesici, A. (2018). Lise öğrencilerinin sorumluluk düzeylerinin çeşitli değişkenlere göre incelenmesi. Gazi Üniversitesi Gazi Eğitim Fakültesi Dergisi, 38 (3), 965–985.

Kesici, A. (2020). The effect of conscientiousness and gender on digital game addiction in high schoolstudents. Journal of Education and Future, 18 , 43–53.

Kesici, Ş, et al. (2003). Temel psikolojik ihtiyaçlar ölçeğinin geçerlik ve güvenirliği . Malatya, Turkey: VII. National PDR Congress İnönü University.

Kilinç, Z., & Gürer, B. (2019). Doğa sporları yapanların temel psikolojik ihtiyaçlarının zihinsel dayanıklılığa etkisi. CBÜ Beden Eğitimi ve Spor Bilimleri Dergisi, 14 (2), 222–233. https://doi.org/10.33459/cbubesbd.576242

Kim, D., Nam, J. K., & Keum, C. (2022). Adolescent internet gaming addiction and personality characteristics by game genre. PloS One, 17 (2), e0263645. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0263645

King, D. L., & Delfabbro, P. H. (2014). The cognitive psychology of internet gaming disorder. Clinical Psychology Review, 34 (4), 298–308. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2014.03.006

King, L. A., & Hicks, J. A. (2021). The science of meaning in life. Annual Review of Psychology, 72 , 561–584. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-072420-122921

Király, O., Urbán, R., Griffiths, M. D., Ágoston, C., Nagygyörgy, K., Kökönyei, G., & Demetrovics, Z. (2015). The mediating effect of gaming motivation between psychiatric symptoms and problematic online gaming: An online survey. Journal of Medical Internet Research, 17 (4), e3515. https://doi.org/10.2196/jmir.3515

Kokkini, V., Tseliou, E., Abakoumkin, G., & Bozatzis, N. (2022). “Immersed in world of warcraft”: A discursive study of identity management talk about excessive online gaming. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 41 (5), 590–612. https://doi.org/10.1177/0261927X211067820

Kuss, D. J., & Billieux, J. (2017). Technological addictions: Conceptualisation, measurement, etiology and treatment. Addictive Behaviors, 64 , 231–233.

Kuss, D. J., Dunn, T. J., Wӧlfling, K., Müller, K. W., Hędzelek, M., & Marcinkowski, J. (2017). Excessive Internet use and psychopathology: the role of coping.  Clinical Neuropsychiatry, 14 (1), 73–81. https://irep.ntu.ac.uk/id/eprint/28364

Larrieu, M., Billieux, J., & Decamps, G. (2022). Problematic gaming and quality of life in online competitive videogame players: Identification of motivational profiles. Addictive Behaviors, 133 , 107363. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2022.107363

Lee, Y. S., Han, D. H., Yang, K. C., Daniels, M. A., Na, C., Kee, B. S., & Renshaw, P. F. (2008). Depression like characteristics of 5HTTPLPR polymorphism and temperament in excessive internet users. Journal of Affective Disorders, 109 , 165–169. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2007.10.020

Lee, J. Y., Ko, D. W., & Lee, H. (2019). Loneliness, regulatory focus, inter-personal competence, and online game addiction: A moderated mediation model. Internet Research, 29 (2), 381–394. https://doi.org/10.1108/IntR-01-2018-0020

Lelonek-Kuleta, B., Bartczuk, R. P., & Wiechetek, M. (2021). Pay for play–Behavioural patterns of pay-to-win gaming. Computers in Human Behavior, 115 , 106592. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2020.106592

Levine, S. L., Brabander, C. J., Moore, A. M., Holding, A. C., & Koestner, R. (2022). Unhappy or unsatisfied: Distinguishing the role of negative affect and need frustration in depressive symptoms over the academic year and during the COVID-19 pandemic. Motivation and Emotion, 46 (1), 126–136. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-021-09920-3

Liang, Q., Yu, C., Xing, Q., Liu, Q., & Chen, P. (2021). The influence of parental knowledge and basic psychological needs satisfaction on peer victimization and internet gaming disorder among Chinese adolescents: A mediated moderation model. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 18 (5), 2397. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph18052397

Liu, D., Wang, Z., Yang, X., Zhang, Y., Zhang, R., & Lin, S. (2021). Perceived autonomy-supportive parenting and internet addiction: Respiratory sinus arrhythmia moderated the mediating effect of basic psychological need satisfaction. Current Psychology, 40 (9), 4255–4264. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-019-00485-6

Luciana, R. P. (2010). One minute more: Adolescent addiction for virtual world. Procedia-Social and Behavioral Sciences, 2 (2), 3706–3710. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.sbspro.2010.03.576

Marcia, J. E. (1980). Identity in adolescence. In J. Adelson (Ed.), Handbook of adolescent psychology (pp. 159–187). Wiley.

Martins, P., Rosado, A., Ferreira, V., & Biscaia, R. (2015). Examining the validity of the personal-social responsibility questionnaire among athletes. Motriz: Revista de Educação Física, 21 (3), 321–328. https://doi.org/10.1590/S1980-65742015000300014

Martins, P., Rosado, A., Ferreira, V., & Biscaia, R. (2017). Personal and social responsibility among athletes: The role of self-determination, achievement goals and engagement. Journal of Human Kinetics, 57 (1), 39–50. https://doi.org/10.1515/hukin-2017-0045

Mascaro, N., & Rosen, D. H. (2005). Existential meaning’s role in the enhancement of hope and prevention of depressive symptoms. Journal of Personality, 73 (4), 985–1014. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6494.2005.00336.x

Matsumoto, D. (2009). The Cambridge dictionary of psychology . Cambridge University Press.

McInroy, L. B., & Mishna, F. (2017). Cyberbullying on online gaming platforms for children and youth. Child and Adolescent Social Work Journal, 34 (6), 597–607. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10560-017-0498-0

Mergler, A., & Shield, P. (2016). Development of the Personal Responsibility Scale for adolescents. Journal of Adolescence, 100 (51), 50–57. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2016.05.011

Mills, D. J., & Allen, J. J. (2020). Self-determination theory, internet gaming disorder, and the mediating role of self-control. Computers in Human Behavior, 105 , 106209. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2019.106209

Mills, D. J., Milyavskaya, M., Heath, N. L., & Derevensky, J. L. (2018). Gaming motivation and problematic video gaming: The role of needs frustration. European Journal of Social Psychology, 48 (4), 551–559. https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.2343

Monacis, L., de Palo, V., Griffiths, M. D., & Sinatra, M. (2017). Exploring individual differences in online addictions: The role of identity and attachment. International Journal of Mental Health and Addiction, 15 (4), 853–868. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11469-017-9768-5

Mun, I. B., & Lee, S. (2022). A longitudinal study of the impact of parental loneliness on adolescents’ online game addiction: The mediating roles of adolescents’ social skill deficits and loneliness. Computers in Human Behavior, 136 , 107375. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2022.107375

Oliver, M. B., Bowman, N. D., Woolley, J. K., Rogers, R., Sherrick, B. I., & Chung, M. Y. (2016). Video games as meaningful entertainment experiences. Psychology of Popular Media Culture, 5 (4), 390–405. https://doi.org/10.1037/ppm0000066

Orkibi, H., & Ronen, T. (2017). Basic psychological needs satisfaction mediates the association between self-control skills and subjective well-being. Frontiers in Psychology, 8 , 936. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00936

Özen, Y. (2011). Sorumluluk eğitimi . Nobel Yayın Dağıtım.

Özen, Y. (2013). Sorumluluk duygusu ve davranışı ölçeğinin geliştirilmesi güvenirliği ve geçerliği. Gümüshane University Electronic Journal of the Institute of Social Science, 4 (7), 343–356.

Park, C. L. (2010). Making sense of the meaning literature: An integrative review of meaning making and its effects on adjustment to stressful life events. Psychological Bulletin, 136 , 257–301. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0018301

Piri, Z., Majd, M. A., Bazzazyan, S., & ve Ghamari, M. (2018). The mediating role of coping strategies in relation with psychological needs and internet addiction among college student. International Journal of Applied Behavioral Sciences, 5 (3), 9–17. https://doi.org/10.22037/ijabs.v5i3.24194

Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F. (2008). Asymptotic and resampling strategies for assessing and comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator models. Behavior Research Methods, 40 (3), 879–891.

Purwaningsih, E., & Nurmala, I. (2021). The impact of online game addiction on adolescent mental health: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Open Access Macedonian Journal of Medical Sciences, 9 (F), 260–274. https://doi.org/10.3889/oamjms.2021.6234

Qiu, C., Liu, Q., Yu, C., Li, Z., & Nie, Y. (2022). The influence of meaning in life on children and adolescents’ problematic smartphone use: A three-wave multiple mediation model. Addictive Behaviors, 126 , 107199. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2021.107199

Rajab, A. M., Zaghloul, M. S., Enabi, S., Rajab, T. M., Al-Khani, A. M., Basalah, A., & Saquib, N. (2020). Gaming addiction and perceived stress among Saudi adolescents. Addictive Behaviors Reports, 11 , 100261. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.abrep.2020.100261

Rigby, S., & Ryan, R. M. (2017). Time well spent? Motivation for entertainment media and its eudaimonic aspects through the lens of self-determination theory. L. Reinecke, M.B. Oliver (Eds.),  The Routledge handbook of media use and well-being, international perspectives on theory and research on positive media effects (pp. 34–48). Routledge

Rigby, S., & Ryan, R. M. (2011). Glued to games: How video games draw us in and hold us spellbound: How video games draw us in and hold us spellbound . AbC-CLIo.

Robinson, S. (2009). The nature of responsibility in a professional setting. Journal of Business Ethics, 88 (1), 11–19. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10551-009-0103-3

Rosendo-Rios, V., Trott, S., & Shukla, P. (2022). Systematic literature review online gaming addiction among children and young adults: A framework and research agenda. Addictive Behaviors, 129 , 107238. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2022.107238

Ruyter, D. D. (2002). The virtue of taking responsibility. Educational Philosophy and Theory, 34 (1), 25–35. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-5812.2002.tb00283.x

Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2000). The darker and brighter sides of human existence: Basic psychological needs as a unifying concept. Psychological Inquiry, 11 (4), 319–338. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15327965PLI1104_03

Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2017). Self-determination theory: Basic psychological needs in motivation, development, and wellness . Guilford Publications.

Ryan, R. M., Rigby, C. S., & Przybylski, A. (2006). The motivational pull of video games: A self-determination theory approach. Motivation and Emotion, 30 (4), 344–360. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-006-9051-8

Ryff, C. D. (1989). Happiness is everything, or is it? Explorations on the psychological well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57 , 1069–1081. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.57.6.1069

Sachdeva, A., & Verma, R. (2015). Internet gaming addiction: A technological hazard. International Journal of High Risk Behaviors & Addiction, 4 (4), e26359. https://doi.org/10.5812/ijhrba.26359

Şata, M. (2020). Nicel araştırma yaklaşımları. Oğuz, E. (Eds.), Eğitimde araştırma yöntemleri içinde (p. 77–97). Eğiten Kitap Yayıncılık

Sela, Y., Zach, M., Amichay-Hamburger, Y., Mishali, M., & Omer, H. (2020). Family environment and problematic internet use among adolescents: The mediating roles of depression and fear of missing out. Computers in Human Behavior, 106 , 106226. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2019.106226

Shahzadi, U., Koul, R. B., Haq, M. N. U., & Arshad, M. (2022). Attitude, behavior and responsibility to environmental literacy in education organization: A quantitative assessment. Indian Journal of Economics and Business, 21 (1), 709–716. http://www.ashwinanokha.com/IJEB.php

Shek, D. T., Dou, D., Zhu, X., & Chai, W. (2019). Positive youth development: Current perspectives. Adolescent Health, Medicine and Therapeutics, 10 , 131–141. https://doi.org/10.2147/AHMT.S179946

Shen, C. X., Liu, R. D., & Wang, D. (2013). Why are children attracted to the Internet? The role of need satisfaction perceived online and perceived in daily real life. Computers in Human Behavior, 29 (1), 185–192. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2012.08.004

Steger, M. F. (2012). Experiencing meaning in life. In P. T. P. Wong (Ed.), The human quest for meaning: Theories, research and applications (pp. 165–184). Taylor & Francis.

Steger, M. F., Frazier, P., Oishi, S., & Kaler, M. (2006). The Meaning in Life Questionnaire: Assessing the presence of and search for meaning in life. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 53 (1), 80–93. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0167.53.1.80

Subrahmanyam, K., & Šmahel, D. (2011). Constructing identity online: Identity exploration and self-presentation. In:  Digital youth  (pp. 59–80). Springer

Sun, R., Gao, Q., Xiang, Y., Chen, T., Liu, T., & Chen, Q. (2020). Parent–child relationships and mobile phone addiction tendency among Chinese adolescents: The mediating role of psychological needs satisfaction and the moderating role of peer relationships. Children and Youth Services Review, 116 , 105113. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.childyouth.2020.105113

Tamborini, R., Grizzard, M., Bowman, N. D., Reinecke, L., Lewis, R. J., & Eden, A. (2011). Media enjoyment as need satisfaction: The contribution of hedonic and nonhedonic needs. Journal of Communication, 61 (6), 1025–1042. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-2466.2011.01593.x

Tanhan, F., & Özlem, A. (2015). Siber kimliklerin kişiliğe yansıması: Proteus etki (tanımı, nedenleri ve önlenmesi). Online Journal of Technology Addiction and Cyberbullying, 2 (2), 1–19.

Taylı, A. (2006). Akran yardımcılığı uygulaması aracılığıyla lise öğrencilerinde kişisel ve sosyal sorumluluğun arttırılması. [Unpublished doctoral thesis]. Gazi Üniversitesi, Eğitim Bilimleri Enstitüsü. Ankara

Taylı, A. (2013). Sorumluluğun bazı değişkenler açısından değerlendirilmesi. Muğla Sıtkı Koçman Üniversitesi Sosyal Bilimler Enstitüsü Dergisi , 30, 68–84. https://dergipark.org.tr/en/pub/musbed/issue/23302/248613

Tazegül, Ü. (2014). Sporun kişilik üzerindeki etkisinin araştırılması. Journal of Academic Social Science Studies, 25 , 537–544.

Thatcher, A., Wretschko, G., & Fridjhon, P. (2008). Online flow experiences, problematic Internet use and Internet procrastination. Computers in Human Behavior, 24 (5), 2236–2254.

Turan, M. E. (2021). Empathy and video game addiction in adolescents: Serial mediation by psychological resilience and life satisfaction. International Journal of Progressive Education, 17 (4), 282–296. https://doi.org/10.29329/ijpe.2021.366.17

Wang, Q., Luo, X., Tu, R., Xiao, T., & Hu, W. (2022). COVID-19 information overload and cyber aggression during the pandemic lockdown: The mediating role of depression/anxiety and the moderating role of Confucian responsibility thinking. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 19 (3), 1540. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19031540

Wartberg, L., Kriston, L., & Kammerl, R. (2017). Associations of social support, friends only known through the internet, and health-related quality of life with internet gaming disorder in adolescence. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking, 20 (7), 436–441. https://doi.org/10.1089/cyber.2016.0535

Wei, H. T., Chen, M. H., Huang, P. C., & Bai, Y. M. (2012). The association between online gaming, social phobia, and depression: An internet survey. BMC Psychiatry, 12 (1), 1–7. https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-244X-12-92

Weinstein, N., Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2012). Motivation, meaning, and wellness: A self-determination perspective on the creation and internalization of personal meanings and life goals. In P. T. P. Wong (Ed.), The human quest for meaning (pp. 127–152). Routledge. https://doi.org/10.4324/9780203146286-13

Chapter   Google Scholar  

Widyanto, L., & Griffiths, M. (2006). Internet addiction: A critical review. International Journal Mental Health and Addiction, 4 , 31–51. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11469-006-9009-9

Wong, P. T. (2010). The PURE way for a better marriage . International Network on Personal Meaning.

Wong, P. T. P. (2019). What is the greatest need today? Responsibility is the key to surviving and thriving in dangerous times . Retrieved from:  http://www.drpaulwong.com/what-is-the-greatest-need-todayresponsibility-is-the-key-to-surviving-and-thriving-in-dangerous-times/ . Accessed 30 Nov 2022

Wongpakaran, N., Wongpakaran, T., Pinyopornpanish, M., Simcharoen, S., & Kuntawong, P. (2021). Loneliness and problematic internet use: Testing the role of interpersonal problems and motivation for internet use. BMC Psychiatry, 21 (1), 447. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12888-021-03457-y

World Health Organization (2019). International Classification of Diseases (ICD). Groups that were involved in ICD-11 Revision Process . Retrieved from:  https://www.who.int/standards/classifications/classification-of-diseases . Accessed 30 Nov 2022

Wray-Lake, L., & Syvertsen, A. K. (2011). The developmental roots of social responsibility in childhood and adolescence. New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development, 2011 (134), 11–25. https://doi.org/10.1002/cd.308

Wu, A. M., Lei, L. L., & Ku, L. (2013). Psychological needs, purpose in life, and problem video game playing among Chinese young adults. International Journal of Psychology, 48 (4), 583–590. https://doi.org/10.1080/00207594.2012.658057

Xiao, X., & Zheng, X. (2022). The effect of parental phubbing on depression in Chinese junior high school students: The mediating roles of basic psychological needs satisfaction and self-esteem. Frontiers in Psychology, 13 , 868354. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2022.868354

Yalom, I. D. (2020). Existential psychotherapy . Hachette UK.

Yang, J., Ti, Y., & Ye, Y. (2022). Offline and online social support and short-form video addiction among Chinese adolescents: The mediating role of emotion suppression and relatedness needs. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking, 25 (5), 316–322. https://doi.org/10.1089/cyber.2021.0323

Yee, N. (2006). Motivations for play in online games. CyberPsychology & Behavior, 9 (6), 772–775. https://doi.org/10.1089/cpb.2006.9.772

Yıkılmaz, M., & Demir-Güdül, M. (2015). Üniversite öğrencilerinde yaşam doyumu, yaşamda anlam ve bilinçli farkındalık arasındaki ilişkiler. Ege Eğitim Dergisi, 16 (2), 297–315. https://doi.org/10.12984/eed.09530

Yıldırım, E., & Zeren, Ş. G. (2021). Video game addiction in Turkey: Does it correlate between basic psychological needs and perceived social support? Psycho-Educational Research Reviews, 10 (2), 106–117. From: https://eric.ed.gov/?id=EJ1311681

Yough, M., Gilmetdinova, A., & Finney, E. (2022). Teaching the English language learner at the elementary school: Sense of responsibility in an ill-defined role. Journal of Language, Identity & Education, 21 (2), 99–115. https://doi.org/10.1080/15348458.2020.1791707

Young, K. S., & Rodgers, R. C. (2009). The relationship between depression and internetaddiction. CyberPsychology & Behavior, 1 (1), 25–28. https://doi.org/10.1089/cpb.1998.1.25

Young, K. S. (2009). Internet addiction: The emergence of a new clinical disorder. CyberPsychology & Behavior , 11, 237–244. https://doi.org/10.1089/cpb.1998.1.237

Young-Ju, J., Ae-Kyung, C., Jeong-Jin, K., & ve Min-Yeong, L. (2018). Identification of the structural relationship of basic psychological needs and Facebook addiction and continuance. Journal of the Institute of Internet, Broadcasting and Communication, 16 (1), 183–191. https://doi.org/10.7236/JIIBC.2016.16.1.183

Yu, C., Li, X., & Zhang, W. (2015). Predicting adolescent problematic online game use from teacher autonomy support, basic psychological needs satisfaction, and school engagement: A 2-year longitudinal study. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking, 18 (4), 228–233. https://doi.org/10.1089/cyber.2014.0385

Zeren, ŞG., & ve Can, S. (2019). Ergenlerin akademik erteleme davranışlarını açıklamada internet bağımlığı ve temel psikolojik ihtiyaçların rolü. Çukurova Üniversitesi Eğitim Fakültesi Dergisi, 48 (2), 1012–1040.

Zhang, M. X., Wang, X., Shu, M. Y., & Wu, A. M. (2019). Purpose in life, social support, and internet gaming disorder among Chinese university students: A 1-year follow-up study. Addictive Behaviors, 99 , 106070. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2019.106070

Zhang, S., Feng, R., Fu, Y. N., Liu, Q., He, Y., Turel, O., & He, Q. (2022). The bidirectional relationship between basic psychological needs and meaning in life: A longitudinal study. Personality and Individual Differences, 197 , 111784. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2022.111784

Zhao, H., Li, X., Zhou, J., Nie, Q., & Zhou, J. (2020). The relationship between bullying victimization and online game addiction among Chinese early adolescents: The potential role of meaning in life and gender differences. Children and Youth Services Review, 116 , 105261. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.childyouth.2020.105261

Download references

Acknowledgements

We are grateful to the individuals all participants who participated in this study.

Author information

Authors and affiliations.

Department of Guidance and Psychological Counselling, Ağrı İbrahim Çeçen University, Ağrı, Turkey

Alican Kaya

Department of Guidance and Psychological Counselling, Siirt University, Siirt, Turkey

Department of Guidance and Psychological Counselling, Sakarya University PhD Student, Sakarya, Turkey

Hasan Batmaz

International Gaming Research Unit, Psychology Department, Nottingham Trent University, 50 Shakespeare Street, Nottingham, NG1 4FQ, UK

Mark D. Griffiths

You can also search for this author in PubMed   Google Scholar

Contributions

Study conception/design: AK, HB, NT, and MDG. Data collection: AK, HB, and NT. analysis: AK and HB. Drafting of manuscript: AK, HB, NT, and MDG. Editing: MDG. Statistical expertise: AK and HB. Administrative/technical/material support: HB and HYK.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Mark D. Griffiths .

Ethics declarations

Ethical approval.

All procedures performed in this study involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of University’s Research Ethics Board and with the 1975 Helsinki Declaration.

Informed Consent

Informed consent was obtained from all participants.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare no competing interests except for MDG. MDG’s university has received research funding from Norsk Tipping (the gambling operator owned by the Norwegian Government). MDG has also received funding for a number of research projects in the area of gambling education for young people, social responsibility in gambling and gambling treatment from Gamble Aware (formerly the Responsible Gambling Trust) , a charitable body which funds its research program based on donations from the gambling industry. MDG regularly undertakes consultancy for various gambling companies in the area of player protection and social responsibility in gambling.

Additional information

Publisher's note.

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ .

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Kaya, A., Türk, N., Batmaz, H. et al. Online Gaming Addiction and Basic Psychological Needs Among Adolescents: The Mediating Roles of Meaning in Life and Responsibility. Int J Ment Health Addiction (2023). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11469-022-00994-9

Download citation

Accepted : 15 December 2022

Published : 10 January 2023

DOI : https://doi.org/10.1007/s11469-022-00994-9

Share this article

Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content:

Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article.

Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative

  • Basic psychological needs
  • Online gaming addiction
  • Responsibility
  • Meaning in life
  • Find a journal
  • Publish with us
  • Track your research

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

The association between video gaming and psychological functioning.

\r\nJuliane M. von der Heiden*

  • 1 Department of Psychology, Johannes Gutenberg University Mainz, Mainz, Germany
  • 2 Department of Psychosomatic Medicine, University Medical Center, Mainz, Germany

Video gaming is an extremely popular leisure-time activity with more than two billion users worldwide ( Newzoo, 2017 ). However, the media as well as professionals have underscored the potential dangers of excessive video gaming. With the present research, we aimed to shed light on the relation between video gaming and gamers’ psychological functioning. Questionnaires on personality and psychological health as well as video gaming habits were administered to 2,734 individuals (2,377 male, 357 female, M age = 23.06, SD age = 5.91). Results revealed a medium-sized negative correlation between problematic video gaming and psychological functioning with regard to psychological symptoms, affectivity, coping, and self-esteem. Moreover, gamers’ reasons for playing and their preferred game genres were differentially related to psychological functioning with the most notable findings for distraction-motivated players as well as action game players. Future studies are needed to examine whether these psychological health risks reflect the causes or consequences of video gaming.

Introduction

Video gaming is a very popular leisure activity among adults ( Pew Research Center, 2018 ). The amount of time spent playing video games has increased steadily, from 5.1 h/week in 2011 to 6.5 h/week in 2017 ( The Nielsen Company, 2017 ). Video gaming is known to have some benefits such as improving focus, multitasking, and working memory, but it may also come with costs when it is used heavily. By spending a predominant part of the day gaming, excessive video gamers are at risk of showing lower educational and career attainment, problems with peers, and lower social skills ( Mihara and Higuchi, 2017 ). On the one hand, video game use is widespread, and it may come with certain precursors as well as consequences. On the other hand, little is known about the relations between various video gaming habits and psychological functioning. This study aims to shed light on these important relations using a large sample.

A video game is defined as “a game which we play thanks to an audiovisual apparatus and which can be based on a story” ( Esposito, 2005 ). In the last few years, the amount of scientific research devoted to video game playing has increased (e.g., Ferguson, 2015 ; Calvert et al., 2017 ; Hamari and Keronen, 2017 ). Most scientific studies in this area of research have focused on the extent of video game play and its diverse correlates. While some researchers have emphasized the benefits of game playing and even suggested a therapeutic use of video games ( Primack et al., 2012 ; Granic et al., 2014 ; Colder Carras et al., 2018 ), others have been intrigued by its potential dangers ( Anderson et al., 2010 ; Müller and Wölfling, 2017 ).

Parents and professionals may be worried about their excessively playing children being “addicted.” However, problematic and potentially addictive video game use goes beyond the extent of playing (in hours per week; Skoric et al., 2009 ). It also includes such issues as craving, loss of control, and negative consequences of excessive gaming. While it is still a matter of debate whether problematic video game play should be considered a behavioral addiction , its status as a mental disorder has been clarified since the release of the DSM-5 in 2013. In the DSM-5, the American Psychiatric Association (2013) defined Internet Gaming Disorder with diagnostic criteria closely related to Gambling Disorder. Generally, this decision has been supported by many researchers (e.g., Petry et al., 2014 ) but has also caused controversies. Researchers have criticized the selection of diagnostic criteria and the vague definition of the Internet Gaming Disorder construct, which excludes offline games from being related to addictive use (e.g., Griffiths et al., 2016 ; Bean et al., 2017 ).

Several studies, literature reviews, and meta-analyses have focused on the correlates of problematic video gaming, usually assessed as a continuum with addiction marking the upper end of the scale (e.g., Ferguson et al., 2011 ; Kuss and Griffiths, 2012 ). The degree of addictive video game use has been found to be related to personality traits such as low self-esteem ( Ko et al., 2005 ) and low self-efficacy ( Jeong and Kim, 2011 ), anxiety, and aggression ( Mehroof and Griffiths, 2010 ), and even to clinical symptoms of depression and anxiety disorders ( Wang et al., 2018 ). Potential consequences of video game use have been identified as well, such as a lack of real-life friends ( Kowert et al., 2014a ), stress and maladaptive coping ( Milani et al., 2018 ), lower psychosocial well-being and loneliness ( Lemmens et al., 2011 ), psychosomatic problems ( Müller et al., 2015 ; Milani et al., 2018 ), and decreased academic achievement ( Chiu et al., 2004 ; Gentile, 2009 ). Effect sizes have varied widely across studies ( Ferguson et al., 2011 ). There seem to be sex and age differences with regard to video gaming behavior: potentially problematic video gaming was found to be more likely among males than females (e.g., Greenberg et al., 2010 ; Estévez et al., 2017 ), and among younger gamers ( Rehbein et al., 2016 ).

In addition to looking at problematic video game use and its relation to psychological functioning, it is relevant to also focus on why individuals play video games. Players use video games for very different reasons ( Ryan et al., 2006 ; Yee, 2006 ) such as to distract themselves from daily hassles or because they enjoy the social relationships they have developed in the virtual world. Potentially problematic video gaming has been found to be related to various reasons for playing such as coping and escape ( Hussain and Griffiths, 2009 ; Schneider et al., 2018 ), socialization ( Laconi et al., 2017 ), and personal satisfaction ( Ng and Wiemer-Hastings, 2005 ). Coping ( Laconi et al., 2017 ), social interaction, and competition were among the main reasons for gaming among males but not among females ( Lucas and Sherry, 2004 ). Mixed results emerged concerning age differences ( Greenberg et al., 2010 ), but especially younger gamers seemed to be motivated for video gaming by social interactions ( Hilgard et al., 2013 ). However, so far it remains unclear to what extent people’s various reasons for playing video games are differentially related to their psychological functioning.

Besides investigating the links between potentially problematic video game use and psychological functioning as well as between reasons for playing video games and psychological functioning, it is relevant to also look at which game genres individuals prefer. Correlates of preferences for certain game genres (e.g., simulation, strategy, action, role-playing) are cognitive enhancement ( Dobrowolski et al., 2015 ; Bediou et al., 2018 ), but also the amount of time spent playing ( Lemmens and Hendriks, 2016 ; Rehbein et al., 2016 ) and psychopathological symptoms ( Laconi et al., 2017 ). Males were shown to prefer action and strategy games, whereas females showed a preference for games of skill ( Scharkow et al., 2015 ; Rehbein et al., 2016 ). Younger gamers seemed to prefer action games, older players more so games of skill ( Scharkow et al., 2015 ). However, it is not yet understood to what extent preferences for certain video game genres are differentially related to psychological functioning.

Typically, research has focused merely on violent video games (e.g., Anderson and Bushman, 2001 ; Elson and Ferguson, 2014 ) or one specific game within one specific game genre (frequently World of Warcraft; Graham and Gosling, 2013 ; Visser et al., 2013 ; Herodotou et al., 2014 ), thereby neglecting the variety of possible gaming habits across various game genres.

In the present study, our objective was to examine the relation between video gaming and psychological functioning in a fine-grained manner. For this purpose, we examined psychological functioning by employing various variables such as psychological symptoms, coping strategies, and social support. Likewise, we assessed video gaming in a similarly detailed way, ranging from (a) problematic video game use, (b) the reasons for playing, to (c) the preferred game genres. This strategy prevented us from making potentially invalid generalizations about video gaming in general and allowed us to examine the spectrum of gaming habits and the respective relations between such habits and a diverse set of variables representing psychological functioning.

Playing video games excessively should be appealing to individuals with poor psychological functioning because games allow people to avoid their everyday problems and instead immerse themselves in another environment ( Taquet et al., 2017 ). Moreover, video games offer people a chance to connect with other people socially despite any more or less evident psychological problems they may have ( Kowert et al., 2014b ; Mazurek et al., 2015 ). On the other hand, potentially problematic video game use may also lead to psychological problems because it reduces the amount of time and the number of opportunities gamers have to practice real-life behavior ( Gentile, 2009 ). Thus, we expected to find a negative correlation between problematic video gaming and variables representing psychological functioning such that we expected more potentially problematic video game use to be related to dysfunctional coping strategies ( Wood and Griffith, 2007 ), negative affectivity ( Mathiak et al., 2011 ), and poor school performance ( Mihara and Higuchi, 2017 ). Moreover, we expected to find differential correlates of people’s reasons for playing video games and their psychological functioning: Playing for escape-oriented reasons such as distraction should go along with diverse indices of poor psychological functioning ( Király et al., 2015 ), whereas playing for gain-oriented reasons such as the storyline or the social connections in the game should be related to adequate psychological functioning ( Longman et al., 2009 ). Also, we expected to find people’s preferred game genres (e.g., strategy, action) to be differentially related to their psychological functioning ( Park et al., 2016 ). Finally, we aimed to shed light on the unique contribution of each measure of psychological functioning to the prediction of problematic video game use.

Materials and Methods

Participants 1.

A total of N = 2,891 individuals (2,421 male, 470 female) with a mean age of 23.17 years ( SD = 5.99, Range: 13–65) participated in our study. Of these participants, N = 2,734 (95%) confirmed their use of video games and were thus included in further analyses (2,377 male, 357 female, with a mean age of 23.06 years; SD = 5.91, Range: 13–65). The distribution of participants with regard to sex and age mirrors the findings of past research with males and younger individuals being more likely to play video games (e.g., Griffiths et al., 2004 ). Participants’ place of residence was Germany.

Procedure and Instruments 2

We posted links to our online questionnaire on various online forums as well as on popular online game sites. To achieve heterogeneity of the sample, no exclusion criteria other than having access to the Internet and understanding German were specified. As an incentive to participate in the study, four vouchers of 50€ were raffled.

Video Gaming

Potentially problematic video game use.

The AICA-S, the Scale for the Assessment of Internet and Computer game Addiction ( Wölfling et al., 2016 ), was used to assess participants’ gaming behavior with regard to potential problematic use. Based on the DSM criteria for Internet Gaming Disorder (tolerance, craving, loss of control, emotion regulation, withdrawal, and unsuccessful attempts to cut back), this standardized self-report scale consists of 15 items usually with a five-point scale ranging from 1 ( never ) to 5 ( very often ). The final score (Min = 0, Max = 27 points) is computed using weighted scoring (items with an item-total correlation > 0.55 in the norm sample are weighted double; Wölfling et al., 2011 ). The AICA-S score can be used to differentiate between regular (0–6.5 points) and problematic use of video games (7–13 points: abuse; 13.5–27 points: addiction). In our sample, N = 2,265 (83%) were identified as regular gamers, and N = 469 (17%) as problematic gamers. We used the AICA-S as a continuous variable for all further analyses ( M = 3.98, SD = 3.22, Range: 0–24). The instrument has been validated for different age groups in the general population and in clinical samples ( Müller et al., 2014a , 2019 , but note small sample size; Müller et al., 2014b ). Cronbach’s alpha was α = 0.70. As expected, the AICA-S score was correlated with male sex ( r = 0.17 ∗∗∗ ) and age ( r = –0.15 ∗∗∗ ). On average, participants played video games for M = 4.09 hours per weekday ( SD = 4.44, Range: 0–24), and M = 4.21 h per day at the weekend ( SD = 2.99, Range: 0–24).

Reasons for playing

Gamers indicated how often they played video games for certain reasons. They rated each of 10 reasons separately on Likert scales ranging from 1 ( never ) to 4 ( very often ). The most prevalent reasons were relaxation ( M = 2.96, SD = 0.91), amusement ( M = 2.94, SD = 0.85), and because of the storyline ( M = 2.67, SD = 1.10).

Game genres

Gamers were asked how often they usually played various video game subgenres such as first-person shooter, round-based strategy, massively multiplayer online role-playing games (MMORPGs), life simulations, and others. Ratings were made on Likert scales ranging from 1 ( never ) to 4 ( very often ). Using Apperley’s (2006) classification of game genres, we categorized the subgenres into the main genres action ( M = 2.54, SD = 0.84), strategy ( M = 2.13, SD = 0.80), role-playing ( M = 2.01, SD = 0.73), and simulation ( M = 1.58, SD = 0.44). A cluster for unclassified subgenres ( M = 1.54, SD = 0.39) was added to additionally account for such subgenres as jump’n’runs and games of skill. Descriptive statistics and intercorrelations for all measures (including sex and age) are presented in Supplementary Tables S1–S4 .

Psychological Functioning

Participants provided ratings of their psychological functioning on the following constructs:

General psychopathology

The SCL-K-9 ( Klaghofer and Brähler, 2001 ), a short version of the SCL-90-R ( Derogatis, 1975 ), was administered to assess participants’ subjective impairment regarding psychological symptoms (somatization, obsessive-compulsive, interpersonal sensitivity, depression, anxiety, hostility, phobic anxiety, paranoid ideation, and psychoticism). The SCL-K-9 score is strongly correlated with the original score of the SCL-90-R ( r = 0.93). The 9 items were answered on 5-point Likert-type scales ranging from 1 ( do not agree at all ) to 5 ( agree completely ). Cronbach’s alpha was satisfactory (α = 0.77).

We assessed 10 coping strategies with the Brief COPE ( Carver, 1997 ; German version by Knoll et al., 2005 ), which is the shorter version of the COPE ( Carver et al., 1989 ): self-distraction, denial, substance use, venting, self-blame, behavioral disengagement, acceptance, active coping, planning, and positive reframing. The two items per subscale were administered on 5-point Likert-type scales ranging from 1 ( never ) to 5 ( very often ). Intercorrelations of the two items per subscale ranged from r = 0.32, p < 0.001 for positive reframing to r = 0.78, p < 0.001 for substance use (with one exception: r = -0.05, p = 0.01 for self-distraction).

We measured general affect as a trait and affect during video gaming as a state using the German version ( Krohne et al., 1996 ) of the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS; Watson et al., 1988 ). On a 5-point Likert-type scale ranging from 1 ( not at all ) to 5 ( completely ), participants rated the intensity of 20 adjectives. Cronbach’s alpha was α = 0.78 for general positive affect, α = 0.83 for general negative affect, α = 0.85 for positive affect while playing, and α = 0.83 for negative affect while playing.

The measure for the assessment of shyness in adults ( Asendorpf, 1997 ) consists of 5 items that were answered on a 5-point Likert-type scale ranging from 1 ( not at all ) to 5 ( completely ). Cronbach’s alpha was excellent (α = 0.86).

We administered the German version ( Elbing, 1991 ) of the NYU Loneliness Scale ( Rubenstein and Shaver, 1982 ). The 4 items were answered on 5- to 6-point Likert-type scales. Cronbach’s alpha was satisfactory (α = 0.79).

Preference for solitude

A 10-item measure of preference for solitude ( Nestler et al., 2011 ) was answered on a 6-point Likert-type scale ranging from 1 ( not at all ) to 6 ( completely ). Cronbach’s alpha was excellent (α = 0.86).

Life satisfaction

Participants answered a one-item life satisfaction measure on a 4-point Likert-type scale ranging from 1 ( not at all ) to 4 ( completely ).

Self-esteem

We administered the German version ( von Collani and Herzberg, 2003 ) of the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale (RSES; Rosenberg, 1979 ). The 10 items were answered on a 4-point Likert-type scale ranging from 1 ( not at all ) to 4 ( completely ). Cronbach’s alpha was excellent (α = 0.88).

Self-efficacy

We administered a 10-item generalized self-efficacy scale ( Schwarzer and Jerusalem, 1995 ), which was answered on a 4-point Likert-type scale ranging from 1 ( not at all ) to 4 ( completely ). Cronbach’s alpha was excellent (α = 0.86).

Social support and friends

We administered the perceived available social support subscale from the Berlin Social Support Scales (BSSS; Schwarzer and Schulz, 2003 ). The 8 items were answered on a 5-point Likert-type scale ranging from 1 ( not at all ) to 5 ( completely ). Cronbach’s alpha was excellent (α = 0.94). Participants indicated how many offline friends and offline acquaintances they had ( r = 0.44, p < 0.001) as well as how many online friends and online acquaintances they had ( r = 0.33, p < 0.001). Due to left-skewed distributions, we logarithmized the data before aggregation.

Participants reported their grade point average. German grades are assessed on a scale that ranges from 1 ( excellent ) to 6 ( insufficient ). Thus, higher scores indicate worse grades.

Participants further reported their sex and age. Both were used as control variables in further analyses.

In a first step, we computed zero-order correlations between the video gaming variables and the measures of psychological functioning. In a second step, we computed partial correlations in which we controlled for sex and age because past research has repeatedly shown that sex and age are correlated with both video gaming ( Homer et al., 2012 ; Mihara and Higuchi, 2017 ) and psychological functioning ( Kessler et al., 2007 ; Nolen-Hoeksema, 2012 ). Finally, we explored the unique contribution of each measure of psychological functioning to the prediction of potentially problematic video gaming. Therefore, we computed regressions with potentially problematic video gaming as the dependent variable and sex, age, and the measures of psychological functioning as predictors (entered simultaneously into the regression equation). By employing this procedure, we were able to determine the effect that each variable had over and above the other ones. For instance, we could identify whether general psychopathology was predictive of potentially problematic video game use when the influence of all other variables (e.g., shyness, loneliness, and others) was held constant.

Additionally, we included analyses regarding sex and age differences in the link between video gaming and psychological functioning. Since we collected a self-selected sample where different sexes and age groups were not represented equally, our findings are only preliminary, but may stimulate future research.

Potentially Problematic Video Game Use and Psychological Functioning

First, we examined whether potentially problematic video game use was related to various psychological functioning variables. As can be seen in Table 1 , the results for the zero-order correlations were similar to those for the partial correlations in which we controlled for sex and age. A medium-sized positive relation to the potentially problematic use of video games emerged for the presence of psychological symptoms including depression, anxiety, and hostility. Furthermore, several coping strategies were differentially associated with the potentially problematic use of video games: Self-blame and behavioral disengagement showed the strongest positive relations to potentially problematic video game use, followed by denial, acceptance, substance use, self-distraction, and venting. Planning, active coping, and, to a lesser extent, positive reframing were negatively associated with the potentially problematic use of video games. Moreover, the association with potentially problematic video game use was negative for general positive affect and positive and larger in size for general negative affect. However, potentially problematic video game use was clearly positively associated with the experience of both positive and negative affect while playing. Further, a preference for solitude, shyness, and loneliness were positively correlated with the potentially problematic use of video games. Lower self-esteem, lower life satisfaction, and, to a lesser extent, poorer perceived social support and lower self-efficacy went along with potentially problematic video game use. There was an association between fewer offline friends and acquaintances but more online connections with potentially problematic video gaming. Finally, poorer performance in school (i.e., higher grades) was related to the potentially problematic use of video games. These results suggest that potentially problematic video gaming goes along with poor psychological functioning and vice versa.

www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Associations between potentially problematic video gaming and psychological functioning.

Reasons for Playing Video Games and Psychological Functioning

Second, we investigated whether players’ reasons for playing video games were differentially related to the psychological functioning variables. Table 2 presents the partial correlations, controlling for sex and age. Using video games to distract oneself from stress was clearly connected to a high level of psychological symptoms. Distraction-motivated gamers preferred coping strategies such as self-blame, behavioral disengagement, self-distraction, denial, substance use, venting, and acceptance, but they neglected active coping and planning. They showed less general positive affect and more negative affect both in general and while playing as well as more positive affect while playing. These gamers further reported low self-esteem and low life satisfaction, loneliness, a preference for solitude, shyness, a lack of self-efficacy and social support, and poor achievement in school. A similar but somewhat less extreme picture was revealed for gamers who played video games in order to have something to talk about . However, these gamers reported more online connections. Gamers who played video games to improve their real-life abilities also reported more online connections. In addition, these gamers showed higher levels of general positive affect. The strongest association with online friends and acquaintances emerged, as expected, for gamers who played because of the social relations in the virtual world. Although all reasons for playing video games were related to positive affect while playing, the strongest associations emerged for gamers who played because of the social relations , to stimulate their imagination , and for curiosity . It is interesting that, for gamers who played video games because of the storyline and for relaxation , there was a relation only to positive but not to negative affect while playing. Reasons for playing were only weakly related to sex and age (see Supplementary Table S2 ). In sum, several reasons for playing video games were differentially associated with psychological functioning.

www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Associations between reasons for playing video games and psychological functioning.

Video Game Genre and Psychological Functioning

Third, we examined whether players’ preferences for different video game genres were differentially associated with the measures of psychological functioning. Table 3 shows the partial correlations in which we controlled for sex and age. There was a weak connection between general psychological symptoms and all of the video game genres we investigated except strategy. A preference for action games had the strongest association with affect while playing. Thus, action games seem to be both rewarding and a source of frustration. A preference for action games went along with poorer school performance. Gamers who preferred role-playing games scored higher on shyness and a preference for solitude and lower on self-esteem; they also reported fewer offline connections. By contrast, preferences for games of the unclassified category on average went along with a larger number of offline friends and more positive affect, both while playing and in general. Two game genres (i.e., role-playing and unclassified games) were related to the coping strategy of self-distraction. Because preferred game genre was related to participants’ sex (see Supplementary Table S3 ), we had a more detailed look at the correlations between preferred game genre and psychological functioning separately for both sexes: For males ( n = 2,377), the strongest correlation between general psychopathology and game genre emerged for action ( r = 0.08, p < 0.001), followed by role playing ( r = 0.07, p < 0.01), and unclassified ( r = 0.07, p < 0.01). For females ( n = 357), the strongest relation between general psychopathology and game genre emerged for simulation ( r = 0.17, p < 0.01). Differences were also found regarding the strength of the relation between number of friends online and the genre action: r = 0.06, p < 0.01 for males, and r = 0.27, p < 0.001 for females. Similarly, preferred game genre was related to participants’ age (see Supplementary Table S3 ). However, there were merely differences with regard to the relation of psychological functioning and game genre, when analyzed separately for different age groups (<19 years, n = 557; 19–30 years, n = 1916; >31 years, n = 261). In sum, our results speak to the idea that individuals with different levels of psychological functioning differ in their choices of game genres and vice versa.

www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Associations between preferred video game genre and psychological functioning.

Predicting Potentially Problematic Video Game Use by Psychological Functioning Variables

In a final step, we entered all of the investigated psychological functioning variables as well as sex and age as predictors of the potentially problematic use of video games. By employing this procedure, we were able to determine the unique contribution of each psychological functioning variable when the influence of all other variables was held constant. As Table 4 shows, the number of online friends and acquaintances as well as positive affect while playing were most predictive of potentially problematic video game use over and above all other variables. General psychopathology, a lack of offline connections, and poor school performance were weaker but still relevant predictors of potentially problematic video game use.

www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Prediction of potentially problematic video game use by psychological functioning variables.

With this study, we aimed to shed light on the association of diverse video gaming habits with gamers’ psychological functioning. Drawing on a large sample, our results revealed a medium-sized relation between potentially problematic video game use and poor psychological functioning with regard to general psychological symptoms, maladaptive coping strategies, negative affectivity, low self-esteem, and a preference for solitude as well as poor school performance. These findings are in line with those of prior work (e.g., Kuss and Griffiths, 2012 ; Milani et al., 2018 ). Also, reasons for playing video games were differentially related to psychological functioning with the most pronounced findings for escape-oriented in contrast to gain-oriented motives. Specifically, distraction-motivated gaming went along with higher symptom ratings, lower self-esteem, and more negative affectivity, whereas playing to establish social relationships in the virtual world was related to a larger number of online connections and more positive affect while playing. Furthermore, there were only weak relations between the preferred game genres and psychological functioning. The action games genre was associated with the strongest ratings of affect while playing. These results on reasons and genres may help to explain conflicting findings of former studies, because in our work we examined various reasons for playing, several game genres, and various aspects of psychological functioning simultaneously. Finally, positive affect while playing and a larger number of online friends were the strongest unique predictors of potentially problematic video game use, followed by psychological symptoms, a lack of offline connections, and poor school performance. These findings suggest that, on the one hand, independent of one’s psychological conditions, enjoying oneself during gaming (i.e., experiencing positive affect, connecting with online friends) may go along with potentially problematic use of video games. On the other hand, poor psychological functioning seems to be a unique risk factor for potentially problematic video gaming.

The presented results are generally in line with previous work that has identified a connection between video gaming and psychological health, academic problems, and social problems ( Ferguson et al., 2011 ; Müller et al., 2015 ). However, our study moved beyond prior research by providing in-depth analyses of both video gaming habits (including potentially problematic use, reasons for playing, and preferred game genre) and psychological functioning (including psychological symptoms, coping styles, affectivity, as well as variables that are related to individuals and their social environments). In addition, we identified unique predictors of potentially problematic video game use.

How can the findings on differential relations between video gaming and various indices of psychological functioning – ranging from beneficial results ( Latham et al., 2013 ) to unfavorable results ( Barlett et al., 2009 ; Möller and Krahé, 2009 ; Anderson et al., 2010 ) – be integrated? According to Kanfer and Phillips (1970) , problematic behavior (e.g., excessive video gaming) can be understood as a function of the situation (e.g., being rejected by a peer); the organism (e.g., low self-esteem); the person’s thoughts, physical reactions, and feelings (e.g., sadness, anger); and finally, the short- as well as long-term consequences of the behavior (termed SORKC model). In the short run, according to our results, playing video games may be a way to distract oneself from everyday hassles and may lead to positive affect while playing and a feeling of being connected to like-minded people, all of which are factors that have an immediate reinforcing value. In the long run, however, spending many hours per day in front of a computer screen may prevent a person from (a) developing and practicing functional coping strategies, (b) finding friends and support in the social environment, and (c) showing proper school achievement, factors that are potentially harmful to the person. Thus, differentiating between short- and long-term perspectives may help us understanding the differential correlates of intensive video gaming.

When is it appropriate to speak of video game addiction? More and more researchers have suggested a continuum between engagement ( Charlton and Danforth, 2007 ; Skoric et al., 2009 ) and pathological gaming/addiction, instead of a categorical perspective. In part, this recommendation has also been followed in the DSM-5 ( American Psychiatric Association, 2013 ) where Internet Gaming Disorder is classified with different degrees of severity, ranging from mild to moderate to severe, according to the functional impairment associated with it. The AICA-S also allows for a differential perspective on gaming behavior by providing ways to assess both the time spent playing video games and the main DSM criteria that indicate Internet Gaming Disorder. However, in our study we did not aim at making a diagnosis, but at having a closer look at potentially problematic gaming behavior and its correlates in a non-clinical sample.

In sum, it seems relevant to assess not only the extent of video game use but also the reasons behind this behavior (e.g., distraction) and the concrete rewards that come from playing (e.g., the experience of strong affect while playing action games) to fully understand the relation between video gaming and psychological functioning.

Limitations and Future Directions

With the present study, we aimed to uncover the association between video gaming and psychological functioning. Our approach was cross-sectional and warrants interpretative caution because correlations cannot determine the direction of causation. It remains unclear whether potentially problematic gaming is a factor that contributes to the development of psychological dysfunction or whether psychological dysfunction contributes to potentially problematic gaming. Also, a third factor (e.g., preexisting mental difficulties) may produce both psychological dysfunction and potentially problematic gaming. Thus, longitudinal studies that are designed to identify the causal pathway may provide a promising avenue for future research. Future studies may also answer the question whether the link between video gaming and psychological functioning is moderated by sex, age, the reasons for playing, or the preferred game genre. In addition, it is important not to forget that the present results are based on a self-selected sample in which potentially problematic video gamers were overrepresented (e.g., Festl et al., 2013 , for a representative sample). Thus, future research should replicate our findings in a representative sample. Further, we relied on self-reported data, which is a plausible method for assessing inner affairs such as people’s reasons for their behaviors, but it would be helpful to back up our findings with evidence derived from sources such as peers, caregivers, and health specialists. Our work reflects only a first approach to the topic, and future work may additionally collect in-game behavioral data from the players ( McCreery et al., 2012 ; Billieux et al., 2013 ) to objectively and more specifically investigate diverse patterns of use. Furthermore, one must not forget that the used taxonomy to classify video game genres is only one of various possible options and one should “think of each individual game as belonging to several genres at once” ( Apperley, 2006 , p. 19). Finally, some of the effects reported in our paper were rather modest in size. This is not surprising considering the complexity and multiple determinants of human behavior. In our analyses, we thoroughly controlled for the influence of sex and age and still found evidence that video gaming was differentially related to measures of psychological functioning.

The current study adds to the knowledge on gaming by uncovering the specific relations between video gaming and distinct measures of psychological functioning. Potentially problematic video gaming was found to be associated with positive affect and social relationships while playing but also with psychological symptoms, maladaptive coping strategies, negative affectivity, low self-esteem, a preference for solitude, and poor school performance. Including gamers’ reasons for playing video games and their preferred game genres helped deepen the understanding of the specific and differential associations between video gaming and psychological health. This knowledge might help developing adequate interventions that are applied prior to the occurrence of psychological impairments that may go along with potentially problematic video gaming.

Ethics Statement

In our online survey, participants were given information on voluntary participation, risks, confidentiality/anonymity, and right to withdraw. Whilst participants were not signing a separate consent form, consent was obtained by virtue of completion. We implemented agreed procedures to maintain the confidentiality of participant data.

Author Contributions

BB, BE, JH, and KM conceived and designed the study. BB, JH, and KM collected and prepared the data. JH analyzed the data. BE and JH wrote the manuscript.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01731/full#supplementary-material

  • ^ The data were gathered as part of a larger project ( Stopfer et al., 2015 ; Braun et al., 2016 ). However, the analyses in the present article do not overlap with analyses from previous work.
  • ^ Other measures were administered, but they were not relevant to the present research questions and are thus not mentioned in this paper. The data set and analysis script supporting the conclusions of this manuscript can be retrieved from https://osf.io/emrpw/?view_only=856491775efe4f99b407e258c2f2fa8d .

American Psychiatric Association (2013). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders: DSM-5 , 5th Edn. Washington DC: American Psychiatric Association.

Google Scholar

Anderson, C. A., and Bushman, B. J. (2001). Effects of violent video games on aggressive behavior, aggressive cognition, aggressive affect, physiological arousal, and prosocial behavior: a meta-analytic review of the scientific literature. Psychol. Sci. 12, 353–359. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.00366

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Anderson, C. A., Shibuya, A., Ihori, N., Swing, E. L., Bushman, B. J., Sakamoto, A., et al. (2010). Violent video game effects on aggression, empathy, and prosocial behavior in eastern and western countries: a meta-analytic review. Psychol. Bull. 136, 151–173. doi: 10.1037/a0018251

Apperley, T. H. (2006). Genre and game studies: toward a critical approach to video game genres. Simul. Gaming 37, 6–23. doi: 10.1177/1046878105282278

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Asendorpf, J. B. (1997). SGSE-Schüchternheits- und Geselligkeitsskalen für Erwachsene [Scales on shyness and sociality for adults ]. Berlin: Humboldt-Universität.

Barlett, C. P., Anderson, C. A., and Swing, E. L. (2009). Video game effects—Confirmed, suspected, and speculative: a review of the evidence. Simul. Gaming 40, 377–403. doi: 10.1177/1046878108327539

Bean, A. M., Nielsen, R. K. L., van Rooij, A. J., and Ferguson, C. J. (2017). Video game addiction: the push to pathologize video games. Prof. Psychol. Res. Pr. 48, 378–389. doi: 10.1037/pro0000150

Bediou, B., Adams, D. M., Mayer, R. E., Tipton, E., Green, C. S., and Bavelier, D. (2018). Meta-analysis of action video game impact on perceptual, attentional, and cognitive skills. Psychol. Bull. 144, 77–110. doi: 10.1037/bul0000130

Billieux, J., Van der Linden, M., Achab, S., Khazaal, Y., Paraskevopoulos, L., Zullino, D., et al. (2013). Why do you play World of Warcraft? An in-depth exploration of self-reported motivations to play online and in-game behaviours in the virtual world of Azeroth. Comput. Hum. Behav. 29, 103–109. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2012.07.021

Braun, B., Stopfer, J. M., Müller, K. W., Beutel, M. E., and Egloff, B. (2016). Personality and video gaming: comparing regular gamers, non-gamers, and gaming addicts and differentiating between game genres. Comput. Hum. Behav. 55, 406–412. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2015.09.041

Calvert, S. L., Appelbaum, M., Dodge, K. A., Graham, S., Nagayama Hall, G. C., Hamby, S., et al. (2017). The american psychological association task force assessment of violent video games: science in the service of public interest. Am. Psychol. 72, 126–143. doi: 10.1037/a0040413

Carver, C. S. (1997). You want to measure coping but your protocol’s too long: consider the brief cope. Int. J. Behav. Med. 4, 92–100. doi: 10.1207/s15327558ijbm0401_6

Carver, C. S., Scheier, M. F., and Weintraub, J. K. (1989). Assessing coping strategies: a theoretically based approach. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 56, 267–283. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.56.2.267

Charlton, J. P., and Danforth, I. D. (2007). Distinguishing addiction and high engagement in the context of online game playing. Comput. Hum. Behav. 23, 1531–1548. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2005.07.002

Chiu, S. I., Lee, J. Z., and Huang, D. H. (2004). Video game addiction in children and teenagers in Taiwan. Cyberpsychol. Behav. 7, 571–581. doi: 10.1089/cpb.2004.7.571

Colder Carras, M., Van Rooij, A. J., Spruijit-Metz, D., Kvedar, J., Griffiths, M. D., Carabas, Y., et al. (2018). Commercial video games as therapy: a new research agenda to unlock the potential of a global pastime. Front. Psychiatry 8:300. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2017.00300

Derogatis, L. R. (1975). SCL-90-R: Symptom Checklist-90-R: Administration, Scoring, and Procedures Manual. London: NCS Pearson.

Dobrowolski, P., Hanusz, K., Sobczyk, B., Skorko, M., and Wiatrow, A. (2015). Cognitive enhancement in video game players: the role of video game genre. Comput. Hum. Behav. 44, 59–63. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2014.11.051

Elbing, E. (1991). Einsamkeit: Psychologische Konzepte, Forschungsbefunde und Treatmentansätze [Loneliness: Psychological Concepts, Research Findings, and Treatments]. Göttingen: Hogrefe.

Elson, M., and Ferguson, C. J. (2014). Twenty-five years of research on violence in digital games and aggression: empirical evidence, perspectives, and a debate gone astray. Eur. Psychol. 19, 33–46. doi: 10.1027/1016-9040/a000147

Esposito, N. (2005). “A short and simple definition of what a videogame is,” in Proceedings of the DiGRA (Digital Games Research Association) Conference: Changing Views-Worlds in Play , (British Columbia: University of Vancouver).

Estévez, A., Jáuregui, P., Sánchez-Marcos, I., López-González, H., and Griffiths, M. D. (2017). Attachment and emotion regulation in substance addictions and behavioral addictions. J. Behav. Addict. 6, 534–544. doi: 10.1556/2006.6.2017.086

Ferguson, C. J. (2015). Do angry birds make for angry children? A meta-analysis of video game influences on children’s and adolescents’ aggression, mental health, prosocial behavior, and academic performance. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 10, 646–666. doi: 10.1177/1745691615592234

Ferguson, C. J., Coulson, M., and Barnett, J. (2011). A meta-analysis of pathological gaming prevalence and comorbidity with mental health, academic and social problems. J. Psychiatr. Res. 45, 1573–1578. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2011.09.005

Festl, R., Scharkow, M., and Quandt, T. (2013). Problematic computer game use among adolescents, younger and older adults. Addiction 108, 592–599. doi: 10.1111/add.12016

Gentile, D. (2009). Pathological video-game use among youth ages 8 to 18: a national study. Psychol. Sci. 20, 594–602. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02340.x

Graham, L. T., and Gosling, S. D. (2013). Personality profiles associated with different motivations for playing World of Warcraft. Cyberpsychol. Behav. Soc. Netw. 16, 189–193. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2012.0090

Granic, I., Lobel, A., and Engels, R. C. (2014). The benefits of playing video games. Am. Psychol. 69, 66–78. doi: 10.1037/a0034857

Greenberg, B. S., Sherry, J., Lachlan, K., Lucas, K., and Holmstrom, A. (2010). Orientations to video games among gender and age groups. Simul. Gaming 41, 238–259. doi: 10.1177/1046878108319930

Griffiths, M., Van Rooij, A. J., Kardefeldt-Winther, D., Starcevic, V., Király, O., Pallesen, S., et al. (2016). Working towards an international consensus on criteria for assessing internet gaming disorder: a critical commentary on Petry et al. (2014). Addiction 111, 167–175. doi: 10.1111/add.13057

Griffiths, M. D., Davies, M. N., and Chappell, D. (2004). Demographic factors and playing variables in online computer gaming. CyberPsychol. Behav. 7, 479–487. doi: 10.1089/cpb.2004.7.479

Hamari, J., and Keronen, L. (2017). Why do people play games? A meta-analysis. Int. J. Inform. Manag. 37, 125–141. doi: 10.1016/j.ijinfomgt.2017.01.006

Herodotou, C., Kambouri, M., and Winters, N. (2014). Dispelling the myth of the socio-emotionally dissatisfied gamer. Comput. Hum. Behav. 32, 23–31. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2013.10.054

Hilgard, J., Engelhardt, C. R., and Bartholow, B. D. (2013). Individual differences in motives, preferences, and pathology in video games: the gaming attitudes, motives, and experiences scales (GAMES). Front. Psychol. 4:608. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00608

Homer, B. D., Hayward, E. O., Frye, J., and Plass, J. L. (2012). Gender and player characteristics in video game play of preadolescents. Comput. Hum. Behav. 28, 1782–1789. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2012.04.018

Hussain, Z., and Griffiths, M. D. (2009). Excessive use of massively multi-player online role-playing games: a pilot study. Int. J. Mental Health Addict. 7:563. doi: 10.1007/s11469-009-9202-8

Jeong, E. J., and Kim, D. H. (2011). Social activities, self-efficacy, game attitudes, and game addiction. Cyberpsychol. Behav. Soc. Netw. 14, 213–221. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2009.0289

Kanfer, F. H., and Phillips, J. (1970). Learning Foundations of Behavior Therapy. New York, NY: Wiley.

Kessler, R. C., Amminger, G. P., Aguilar-Gaxiola, S., Alonso, J., Lee, S., and Ustun, T. B. (2007). Age of onset of mental disorders: a review of recent literature. Curr. Opin. Psychiatry 20, 359–364. doi: 10.1097/YCO.0b013e32816ebc8c

Király, O., Urbán, R., Griffith, M. D., Ágoston, C., Nagygyörgy, K., Kökönyei, G., et al. (2015). The mediating effect of gaming motivation between psychiatric symptoms and problematic online gaming: an online survey. J. Med. Int. Res. 17:e88. doi: 10.2196/jmir.3515

Klaghofer, R., and Brähler, E. (2001). Konstruktion und teststatistische Prüfung einer Kurzform der SCL-90-R [Construction and statistical evaluation of a short version of the SCL-90-R]. Z. Klin. Psychol. Psychiatr. Psychother. 49, 115–124.

Knoll, N., Rieckmann, N., and Schwarzer, R. (2005). Coping as a mediator between personality and stress outcomes: a longitudinal study with cataract surgery patients. Eur. J. Personal. 19, 229–247. doi: 10.1002/per.546

Ko, C. H., Yen, J. Y., Chen, C. C., Chen, S. H., and Yen, C. F. (2005). Gender differences and related factors affecting online gaming addiction among Taiwanese adolescents. J. Nervous Mental Dis. 193, 273–277. doi: 10.1097/01.nmd.0000158373.85150.57

Kowert, R., Domahidi, E., Festl, R., and Quandt, T. (2014a). Social gaming, lonely life? The impact of digital game play on adolescents’ social circles. Comput. Hum. Behav. 36, 385–390. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2014.04.003

Kowert, R., Domahidi, E., and Quandt, T. (2014b). The relationship between online video game involvement and gaming-related friendships among emotionally sensitive individuals. Cyberpsychol. Behav. Soc. Netw. 17, 447–453. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2013.0656

Krohne, H. W., Egloff, B., Kohlmann, C. W., and Tausch, A. (1996). Untersuchungen mit einer deutschen version der positive and negative affect schedule (PANAS) [Investigations with a German version of the PANAS]. Diagnostica 42, 139–156.

Kuss, D. J., and Griffiths, M. D. (2012). Internet gaming addiction: a systematic review of empirical research. Int. J. Mental Health Addict. 10, 278–296. doi: 10.1007/s11469-011-9318-5

Laconi, S., Pirès, S., and Chabrol, H. (2017). Internet gaming disorder, motives, game genres, and psychopathology. Comput. Hum. Behav. 75, 652–659. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2017.06.012

Latham, A. J., Patston, L. L., and Tippett, L. J. (2013). The virtual brain: 30 years of video-game play and cognitive abilities. Front. Psychol. 4:629. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00629

Lemmens, J. S., and Hendriks, S. J. F. (2016). Addictive online games: examining the relationship between game genres and internet gaming disorder. Cyberpsychol. Behav. Soc. Netw. 19, 270–276. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2015.0415

Lemmens, J. S., Valkenburg, P. M., and Peter, J. (2011). Psychosocial causes and consequences of pathological gaming. Comput. Hum. Behav. 27, 144–152. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2010.07.015

Longman, H., O’Connor, E., and Obst, P. (2009). The effect of social support derived from World of Warcraft on negative psychological symptoms. Cyberpsychol. Behav. 12, 563–566. doi: 10.1089/cpb.2009.0001

Lucas, K., and Sherry, J. L. (2004). Sex differences in video game play: a communication-based explanation. Commun. Res. 31, 499–523. doi: 10.1177/0093650204267930

Mathiak, K. A., Klasen, M., Weber, R., Ackermann, H., Shergill, S. S., and Mathiak, K. (2011). Reward system and temporal pole contributions to affective evaluation during a first person shooter video game. BMC Neurosci. 12:66. doi: 10.1186/1471-2202-12-66

Mazurek, M. O., Engelhardt, C. R., and Clark, K. E. (2015). Video games from the perspective of adults with autism spectrum disorder. Comput. Hum. Behav. 51, 122–130. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2015.04.062

McCreery, M. P., Krach, S. K., Schrader, P. G., and Boone, R. (2012). Defining the virtual self: personality, behavior, and the psychology of embodiment. Comput. Hum. Behav. 28, 976–983. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2011.12.019

Mehroof, M., and Griffiths, M. D. (2010). Online gaming addiction: the role of sensation seeking, self-control, neuroticism, aggression, state anxiety, and trait anxiety. Cyberpsychol. Behav. 13, 313–316. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2009.0229

Mihara, S., and Higuchi, S. (2017). Cross-sectional and longitudinal epidemiological studies of Internet gaming disorder: a systematic review of the literature. Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 71, 425–444. doi: 10.1111/pcn.12532

Milani, L., La Torre, G., Fiore, M., Grumi, S., Gentile, D. A., Ferrante, M., et al. (2018). Internet gaming addiction in adolescence: risk factors and maladjustment correlates. Int. J. Mental Health Addict. 16, 888–904. doi: 10.1007/s11469-017-9750-2

Möller, I., and Krahé, B. (2009). Exposure to violent video games and aggression in German adolescents: a longitudinal analysis. Aggress. Behav. 35, 75–89. doi: 10.1002/ab.20290

Müller, K. W., Beutel, M. E., Dreier, M., and Wölfling, K. (2019). A clinical evaluation of the DSM-5 criteria for internet gaming disorder and a pilot study on their applicability to further Internet-related disorders. J. Behav. Addict. 8, 16–24. doi: 10.1556/2006.7.2018.140

Müller, K. W., Beutel, M. E., and Wölfling, K. (2014a). A contribution to the clinical characterization of internet addiction in a sample of treatment seekers: validity of assessment, severity of psychopathology and type of co-morbidity. Compr. Psychiatry 55, 770–777. doi: 10.1016/j.comppsych.2014.01.010

Müller, K. W., Glaesmer, H., Brähler, E., Wölfling, K., and Beutel, M. E. (2014b). Prevalence of internet addiction in the general population: results from a German population-based survey. Behav. Inform. Technol. 33, 757–766. doi: 10.1080/0144929X.2013.810778

Müller, K. W., Janikian, M., Dreier, M., Wölfling, K., Beutel, M. E., Tzavara, C., et al. (2015). Regular gaming behavior and internet gaming disorder in European adolescents: results from a cross-national representative survey of prevalence, predictors and psychopathological correlates. Eur. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 24, 565–574. doi: 10.1007/s00787-014-0611-2

Müller, K. W., and Wölfling, K. (2017). Both sides of the story: addiction is not a pastime activity: commentary on: scholars’ open debate paper on the World Health Organization ICD-11 Gaming Disorder proposal (Aarseth et al.). J. Behav. Addict. 6, 118–120. doi: 10.1556/2006.6.2017.038

Nestler, S., Back, M. D., and Egloff, B. (2011). Psychometrische Eigenschaften zweier Skalen zur Erfassung interindividueller Unterschiede in der Präferenz zum Alleinsein [Psychometric properties of two scales for the assessment of interindividual differences in preference for solitude]. Diagnostica 57, 57–67. doi: 10.1026/0012-1924/a000032

Newzoo (2017). 2017 Global Games Market Report: Trends, Insights, and Projections Toward 2020. Available at: http://progamedev.net/wp-content/uploads/2017/06/Newzoo_Global_Games_Market_Report_2017_Light.pdf (accessed February 16, 2018).

Ng, B. D., and Wiemer-Hastings, P. (2005). Addiction to the internet and online gaming. Cyberpsychol. Behav. 8, 110–113. doi: 10.1089/cpb.2005.8.110

Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2012). Emotion regulation and psychopathology: the role of gender. Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 8, 161–187. doi: 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-032511-143109

Park, J. H., Han, D. H., Kim, B. N., Cheong, J. H., and Lee, Y. S. (2016). Correlations among social anxiety, self-esteem, impulsivity, and game genre in patients with problematic online game playing. Psychiatry Investig. 13, 297–304. doi: 10.4306/pi.2016.13.3.297

Petry, N. M., Rehbein, F., Gentile, D. A., Lemmens, J. S., Rumpf, H. J., Mossle, T., et al. (2014). An international consensus for assessing internet gaming disorder using the new DSM-5 approach. Addiction 109, 1399–1406. doi: 10.1111/add.12457

Pew Research Center (2018). 5 Facts About Americans and Video Games. Washington, DC: Pew Research Center.

Primack, B. A., Carroll, M. V., McNamara, M., Klem, M. L., King, B., Rich, M. O., et al. (2012). Role of video games in improving health-related outcomes: a systematic review. Am. J. Prevent. Med. 42, 630–638. doi: 10.1016/j.amepre.2012.02.023

Rehbein, F., Staudt, A., Hanslmaier, M., and Kliem, S. (2016). Video game playing in the general adult population of Germany: can higher gaming time of males be explained by gender specific genre preferences? Comput. Hum. Behav. 55, 729–735. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2015.10.016

Rosenberg, M. (1979). Conceiving the Self. New York, NY: Basic Books.

Rubenstein, C., and Shaver, P. R. (1982). “The experience of loneliness,” in Loneliness: A Sourcebook of Current Theory, Research, and Therapy , eds L. A. Peplau and D. Perlman (New York, NY: Wiley-Interscience), 206–223.

Ryan, R. M., Rigby, C. S., and Przybylski, A. (2006). The motivational pull of video games: a self-determination theory approach. Motiv. Emot. 30, 344–360. doi: 10.1007/s11031-006-9051-8

Scharkow, M., Festl, R., Vogelgesang, J., and Quandt, T. (2015). Beyond the “core-gamer”: genre preferences and gratifications in computer games. Comput. Hum. Behav. 44, 293–298. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2014.11.020

Schneider, L. A., King, D. L., and Delfabbro, P. H. (2018). Maladaptive coping styles in adolescents with Internet gaming disorder symptoms. Int. J. Mental Health Addict. 16, 905–916. doi: 10.1007/s11469-017-9756-9

Schwarzer, R., and Jerusalem, M. (1995). “Generalized self-efficacy scale,” in Measures in Health Psychology: A User’s Portfolio. Causal and Control Beliefs , eds J. Weinman, S. Wright, and M. Johnston (Windsor: NFER-NELSON), 35–37.

Schwarzer, R., and Schulz, U. (2003). Soziale Unterstützung bei der Krankheitsbewältigung: die Berliner Social Support Skalen (BSSS) [Social support in coping with illness: the Berlin social support scales]. Diagnostica 49, 73–82. doi: 10.1026//0012-1924.49.2.73

Skoric, M. M., Teo, L. L. C., and Neo, R. L. (2009). Children and video games: addiction, engagement, and scholastic achievement. Cyberpsychol. Behav. 12, 567–572. doi: 10.1089/cpb.2009.0079

Stopfer, J. M., Braun, B., Müller, K. W., and Egloff, B. (2015). Narcissus plays video games. Personal. Individ. Differ. 87, 212–218. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2015.08.011

Taquet, P., Romo, L., Cottencin, O., Ortiz, D., and Hautekeete, M. (2017). Video game addiction: cognitive, emotional, and behavioral determinants for CBT treatment. J. Thér. Comportementale Cogn. 27, 118–128. doi: 10.1016/j.jtcc.2017.06.005

The Nielsen Company (2017). Games 360 U.S. Report. New York, NY: The Nielsen Company.

Visser, M., Antheunis, M. L., and Schouten, A. P. (2013). Online communication and social well-being: how playing World of Warcraft affects players’ social competence and loneliness. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 43, 1508–1517. doi: 10.1111/jasp.12144

von Collani, G., and Herzberg, P. Y. (2003). Eine revidierte Fassung der deutschsprachigen Skala zum Selbstwertgefühl von Rosenberg [A revised German version of Rosenberg‘s self-esteem scale]. Z. Differ. Diagn. Psychol. 24, 3–7. doi: 10.1026/0012-1924/a000032

Wang, H. R., Cho, H., and Kim, D.-J. (2018). Prevalence and correlates of comorbid depression in a nonclinical online sample with DSM-5 internet gaming disorder. J. Affect. Disord. 226, 1–5. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2017.08.005

Watson, D., Clark, L. A., and Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: the PANAS scales. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 54, 1063–1070. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.54.6.1063

Wölfling, K., Beutel, M. E., and Müller, K. W. (2016). “OSV-S – skala zum onlinesuchtverhalten [AICA-S – Scale for the assessment of internet and computer game addiction],” in Diagnostische Verfahren in der Psychotherapie [Diagnostic Measures in Psychotherapy] , eds K. Geue, B. Strauß, and E. Brähler (Göttingen: Hogrefe), 362–366.

Wölfling, K., Müller, K. W., and Beutel, M. (2011). Reliabilität und Validität der Skala zum Computerspielverhalten [Reliability and validity of the scale for the assessment of pathological computer-gaming]. Psychother. Psychosom. Med. Psychol. 61, 216–224. doi: 10.1055/s-0030-1263145

Wood, R. T., and Griffith, M. D. (2007). A qualitative investigation of problem gambling as an escape-based coping strategy. Psychol. Psychother. 80, 107–125. doi: 10.1348/147608306X107881

Yee, N. (2006). Motivations for play in online games. Cyberpsychol. Behav. 9, 772–775. doi: 10.1089/cpb.2006.9.772

Keywords : computer games, video gaming behavior, game genres, coping, psychological health

Citation: von der Heiden JM, Braun B, Müller KW and Egloff B (2019) The Association Between Video Gaming and Psychological Functioning. Front. Psychol. 10:1731. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01731

Received: 14 September 2018; Accepted: 11 July 2019; Published: 26 July 2019.

Reviewed by:

Copyright © 2019 von der Heiden, Braun, Müller and Egloff. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY) . The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Juliane M. von der Heiden, [email protected]

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Video Game Addiction: Consequences and Treatments

Ieee account.

  • Change Username/Password
  • Update Address

Purchase Details

  • Payment Options
  • Order History
  • View Purchased Documents

Profile Information

  • Communications Preferences
  • Profession and Education
  • Technical Interests
  • US & Canada: +1 800 678 4333
  • Worldwide: +1 732 981 0060
  • Contact & Support
  • About IEEE Xplore
  • Accessibility
  • Terms of Use
  • Nondiscrimination Policy
  • Privacy & Opting Out of Cookies

A not-for-profit organization, IEEE is the world's largest technical professional organization dedicated to advancing technology for the benefit of humanity. © Copyright 2024 IEEE - All rights reserved. Use of this web site signifies your agreement to the terms and conditions.

  • Research article
  • Open access
  • Published: 12 December 2020

Video gaming addiction and its association with memory, attention and learning skills in Lebanese children

  • Youssef Farchakh 1 ,
  • Chadia Haddad 2 , 3 ,
  • Hala Sacre 4 ,
  • Sahar Obeid 2 , 4 , 5   na1 ,
  • Pascale Salameh 4 , 6 , 7   na1 &
  • Souheil Hallit   ORCID: orcid.org/0000-0001-6918-5689 1 , 4   na1  

Child and Adolescent Psychiatry and Mental Health volume  14 , Article number:  46 ( 2020 ) Cite this article

37k Accesses

20 Citations

18 Altmetric

Metrics details

Examining whether any association exists between addiction to video games and cognitive abilities in children could inform ongoing prevention and management of any possible harm. The objective of this study was to investigate the associations between addiction to video games, and memory, attention and learning abilities among a sample of Lebanese school children.

This cross-sectional study, conducted between January and May 2019, enrolled 566 school children aged between 9 and 13 years. Three private schools were chosen conveniently for this study. Students were randomly chosen from the list given by the school administration. The students’ parents are those who responded to the questionnaire.

The results showed that higher addiction to video gaming salience was significantly associated with worse episodic memory, problem solving, basic reading skills, written expression skills and worse clinical attention. Higher addiction to video gaming tolerance were significantly associated with worse novel problem solving and worse attention. Higher addiction to video gaming withdrawal were significantly associated with worse attention, factual memory, attention, processing speed, visual spatial organization, sustained sequential processing, working memory, novel problem solving and worse written expression skills.

The results suggest a correlation between addiction to video games and worse memory, attention, as well as cognitive and academic abilities among school children. Those findings indicate the need for more extensive research, and serve to highlight vital next steps needed in future papers, such as identifying predicting factors that could aid in early detection of video gaming addiction in children.

Recent advances in new technologies have made video games the top leisure time occupation for children, who are particularly susceptible for addiction. Currently, video games have become the most famous type played worldwide among children. Nielsen reports that total weekly time spent playing games increased expeditiously from 5.1 to 6.3 h in 2011 and 2013 respectively [ 1 ]. In a study involving children aged between 9 and 12, from 12 different countries, it was found that 8.6 h per day were spent on playing video games [ 2 ]. Importantly, the emerging phenomenon of video game addiction represents a real and potential widespread problem that defies easy solutions and prevention methods, and requires further investigations, especially in the childhood population.

In fact, despite the benefits that video games have, such as socialization and entertainment, empirical and clinical studies have frequently demonstrated that the excessive use of video games may drive to adverse consequences in miscellaneous areas of psychological development and can result in an addiction among a small portion of gamers [ 3 ]. Impaired control over gaming and increasing priority over daily activities and other life interests could manifest as an evidence of gaming addiction [ 4 ]. In its fifth edition, the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-5) identified video game addiction in the form of internet gaming disorder, as a condition in need of supplementary studies [ 5 ]. Additionally, the 11th revision of International Classification of Diseases (ICD-11) defined gaming disorder as a recurrent gaming behavior pattern that carries both offline and online gaming [ 6 ]. Science suggests that addictions arise from a mixture of a genetic predisposition and a repeated exposure to a particular substrate [ 7 ].

Along with that, there has been growing public concerns about potential adverse effects, including the possibility that video games might affect memory in children [ 8 ]: as an important process for comprehension, memory updating and working memory, among other cognitive skills, have usually been reported to be damaged across different behavioral disorder individuals and addictive populations [ 9 ]. Even though there are some studies that investigate the effect of video games on cognitive functions and academic performance in children [ 10 , 11 ], consequences on memory are still a prevalent subject of debate.

Although video gaming is a free time activity, it does cause problems for some children, affecting their attention competencies. Both a meta-analysis and a systematic review conducted by Ho et al. and Carli et al. respectively suggested an association between inattention and internet/gaming addiction [ 12 ]. Moreover, plenty of other studies supported this finding and proved a strong correlation between the severity of inattention in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and internet/gaming addiction [ 13 ], even after regulating the effects of depression and anxiety symptoms, as well as personality traits [ 12 ].

Unsurprisingly, video gaming advancements have led to concerns from parents and educators that the exaggerated amount of time consumed by children on video games, may result in decreased learning and academic abilities [ 11 ]. Handful of studies have generated inconclusive results regarding a possible association between poor school grades and problematic gaming [ 11 , 14 ]. Furthermore, in cross-sectional studies, pathological video gaming has been connected to a range of negative consequences, such as lower school and academic performances, but the need for further research to explore this finding is mandatory [ 11 ]. Moreover, researchers have been attempting to shed light on addiction to video games and its association with children’s development and behaviors. Despite all the efforts made, the literature still lacks sufficient studies on the subject.

In Lebanon, adolescents showed a 43.6% of moderate to severe problematic internet use [ 15 ]. Children are affected by video gaming and its related addiction. According to a Lebanese research study published in 2018, gaming disorder was associated with being younger. Moreover, Lebanese children are witnessing devastating long-term adverse effects on their behavioral, physical and psychological health, due to the rapidly advancing technology, which makes it very essential to focus on this particular Lebanese age group [ 14 ]. From the research point of view, only three Lebanese studies were detected concerning video games addiction [ 14 , 16 , 17 ]. Two of them worked on the development and validation of specific scales for video games addiction and internet gaming disorder [ 15 , 16 ], while the third one explored the relationships between gaming disorders, sleep habits, and academic achievement in Lebanese adolescents [ 14 ]. The last study showed that the pooled prevalence of internet gaming disorder was 9.2% [ 14 ]. Examining whether any association exists between addiction to video games and cognitive abilities in children could inform ongoing prevention and management of any possible harm. The objective of this study was to investigate the associations between addiction to video games, and memory, attention and learning abilities among a sample of Lebanese schoolchildren.

Participants

This study was a cross-sectional, conducted between January and May 2019. Three private schools were chosen conveniently for this study. Students were randomly chosen from the list given by the school administration. All participants between the age of 9 and 13 years of age were eligible to participate. The students’ parents are those who answered the questionnaire. Prior to participation, parents were briefed on the study objectives and methodology, and were assured of the anonymity of their participation. Parents had the right to accept or refuse participation in the study, with no financial compensation provided in return.

Sample size calculation

The Epi info program [Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC), Epi Info™] was employed for the calculation of the required sample size for our study, with a prevalence of internet gaming disorder of 9.2% among 524 Lebanese high school students from a study done by Hawi et al. [ 14 ], with an acceptable margin of error of 5% and design effect of 2. The youth population is estimated to be 585,000 according to the UNDP statistics in Lebanon; the result showed that the biggest required sample size is 256 participants.

The questionnaire was distributed to each student in the classroom to be taken home. Parents filled it within 25 min approximately. The completed questionnaires were collected back and sent for data entry. During the data collection process, the anonymity of the participants was guaranteed.

Questionnaire

The self-administered questionnaire used was in Arabic, the native language of Lebanon. The first part assessed the sociodemographic details of the participants (i.e. age, gender, grade, and father and mother education level). The second part of the questionnaire included the following scales:

Game Addiction Scale for Children (GASC)

The 21-item Gaming Addiction Scale (GASC) is an instrument based on DSM criteria to assess gaming addiction. The seven items in the GAS are rated using a five-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (never) to 5 (very often). A higher score on the GAS indicates more problematic use of online gaming. The scale measures 7 criteria of computer addiction: salience, tolerance, mood modification, withdrawal, relapse, conflict and problems [ 18 ]. In this study, the Cronbach alpha values for the GAS was 0.948.

Children’s Memory Questionnaire (CMQ)

The CMQ is a 36-item questionnaire designed to assess parents’ perceptions of their children’s memory. The CMQ requires parents to assess their child’s memory based on five possible options: 1 = never or almost never happens; 2 = happens less than once a week; 3 = happens once or twice in a week; 4 = happens about once a day; and 5 = happens more than once a day [ 19 ]. Three subscales derived from the total scale representing the episodic memory, visual memory and working memory and attention. The higher the scores, the greater the impairment in the cognition domain [ 19 ]. In this study, the Cronbach alpha values for the episodic memory subscale was 0.888, for the visual memory was 0.770 and for the working memory was 0.845.

Clinical attention problems scale

The scale measures the frequency of activity and attention by asking the parent and teacher to respond to a series of 12 statements and their applicability to their child in the morning and afternoon. Response options range from “not true” (0), “somewhat or sometimes true” (1), “very often” or often true (2). The higher the scores, the greater the attention problems exist [ 20 ]. In this study, the Cronbach alpha values for the clinical attention problem in the morning and in the afternoon were 0.844 and 0.839 respectively.

Learning, Executive and Attention Functioning (LEAF) Scale

The LEAF is a 55 item self-report questionnaire that assesses executive functions, related neurocognitive functions, and academic skills in children and adults. The LEAF evaluates a broad set of core cognitive abilities as well as related cognitive learning and academic abilities. Cognitive areas assessed by the LEAF include attention, processing speed (including visual-spatial organization skills), and sustained sequential processing to achieve goals (e.g., planning and executing goal-directed behavior), working memory, and novel problem-solving. Also, LEAF includes comprehension and concept formation, declarative/factual memory, and academic functioning. The LEAF contains Academic subscales assessing reading, writing, and math fluency and abilities. LEAF items are grouped by subscale, and all subscales have the same number of items.

The subscales of the LEAF are: (1) comprehension and conceptual learning (tracking and understanding information), (2) factual memory (memorization and retention of facts); (3) attention (sustained focus); (4) processing speed (speed of completing cognitive and behavioral tasks that involve a component of focus and concentration); (5) visual-spatial organization (organization and visual-constructive skills); (6) sustained sequential processing (planning and sustaining effort in order to follow and complete multistep directions and sequences); (7) working memory (remembering and processing multiple things at the same time); and (8) novel problem solving (initiating effort toward processing new or unfamiliar information). (9) Mathematics skills (math calculation difficulty); (10) basic reading skills (reading/phonics difficulty); and (11) written expression skills (limited/impoverished or slow/effortful written expression). Individual items are rated on a 0–3 scale, and a raw subscale score for each of the 11 content areas is created by summing the 5 constituent items, such that higher scores indicate more cognitive problems [ 21 ]. In this study, the Cronbach alpha values for the subscales was: comprehension and conceptual learning = 0.961; factual memory = 0.792; attention = 0.901; processing speed = 0.866; visual-spatial organization = 0.729; sustained sequential processing = 0.768; working memory = 0.816; novel problem solving = 0.811; mathematics skills = 0.871; basic reading skills = 0.923 and written expression skills = 0.905.

Translation procedure

The forward translation was done by one translator. An expert committee formed by healthcare professionals and a language professional verified the Arabic translated version. A backward translation was then performed by a second translator, unaware of the initial English version. The back-translated English questionnaire was subsequently compared to the original English one, by the expert committee. Discrepancies related to inadequate expressions and concepts, confusing in meaning and slightly off in meaning during the reconciliation of the back translated questionnaire with the original source were resolved by consensus.

Statistical analysis

SPSS software version 23 was used to conduct data analysis. Cronbach’s alpha values were recorded for reliability analysis for all the scales. A descriptive analysis was done using the counts and percentages for categorical variables and mean and standard deviation for continuous measures. A multivariate analysis of covariance (MANCOVA) was carried out to compare multiple measures (each scale was taken as a dependent variable) taking the GAS as the major independent variable, controlling for potential confounding variables: age, gender, family monthly income, and mother and father education level. A p-value less than 0.05 was considered significant.

Out of 700 distributed questionnaires, 566 (80.86%) questionnaires were completed and collected back. The sociodemographic characteristics of the participants are summarized in Table 1 . The mean age was 10.77 ± 1.38 years, with 55.2% male. Also, higher education level in parents was found in 58.9% among mother and 39.4% among father.

Description of the scales used

The description of all the scales used in terms of mean, standard deviation, median, minimum and maximum is summarized in Table 2 .

Bivariate analysis

The bivariate analysis of the sociodemographic variables associated with the memory and attention scores is summarized in Tables 3 and 4 .

Higher addiction to video gaming salience, tolerance, relapse, withdrawal, conflict and problems scores were significantly correlated with lower/worse episodic memory, working memory, visual memory, clinical attention in the morning and in the afternoon, comprehension and conceptual learning, factual memory, LEAF attention, processing speed, visual spatial organization, sustained sequential processing, LEAF working memory, novel problem solving, mathematics skills, basic reading and written expression skills scores, except for the mood modification that was not associated with the visual memory and mathematics skills scores (Table 5 ).

Multivariate analysis

The MANCOVA analysis was performed taking the scales as the dependent variables and the addiction to video gaming salience as the independent variable, adjusting for the covariates (age, gender, family monthly income, and mother and father education levels).

Higher addiction to video gaming salience was significantly associated with higher episodic memory score (worse episodic memory), higher novel problem solving score (worse problem solving), higher basic reading skills score (worse basic reading skills), higher written expression skills score (worse written expression skills) and higher clinical attention in the morning and afternoon scores (worse episodic memory and attention).

Higher addiction to video gaming tolerance were significantly associated with higher novel problem solving score (worse novel problem solving) and higher clinical attention in the morning and afternoon scores (worse attention).

Higher addiction to video gaming withdrawal were significantly associated with higher clinical attention in the afternoon score (worse attention), higher factual memory score (worse factual memory), higher attention score (worse attention), higher processing speed score (worse processing speed), higher visual spatial organization score (worse visual spatial organization), higher sustained sequential processing score (worse sustained sequential processing), higher working memory score (worse working memory), higher novel problem solving score (worse novel problem solving) and higher written expression skills score (worse written expression skills) (Table 6 ).

Thus far, this is the first study to be conducted on Lebanese school children to evaluate the association between addiction to video games, memory, attention and learning abilities. Taking into consideration that intensive video gaming has frequently been linked to the development of addictive-like behaviors among children, we found highly important to assess some cognitive factors that could be affected in this delicate population. The results showed that higher addiction to video gaming was significantly associated with higher memory score (worse memory), with higher attention score (worse attention), and higher LEAF scale and subscales scores (worse cognitive and academic abilities). We also found that having a father with university education level was significantly associated with lower attention score (better attention) and lower LEAF scale and subscales scores (better cognitive and academic abilities).

Higher addiction to video gaming was significantly associated with higher memory score (worse memory). When the different studies were reviewed, the results were found to be contradictory. Some of the studies argued that video games have negative effects on memory, while other ones did not support this finding [ 22 ]. Moreover, an observational study comparing patients with internet gaming disorder against healthy control groups revealed that the formers had lower working memory functioning [ 8 ]. This particular result regarding memory may relate to the fact that spending too much time playing the same type of games could be harming their abilities to retain and boost memories.

Moreover, higher addiction to video gaming was significantly associated with higher attention score (worse attention). Similar to our results, increasing behavioral studies proved that video games addicts showed attention deficits. De facto, Zhou et al. [ 23 ] demonstrated that when performing the attention-related task, more total error rates was observed in subjects with internet gaming addiction in comparison to controls. To boot, Song et al. [ 24 ] established a positive correlation between inattention and internet gaming addiction severity, while lack of attention was verified to be a high predictor of online gaming addiction by Ko et al. [ 25 ]. The finding we obtained could be interpreted by the instant rewards and constant stimulation of video games that raise the threshold for children to pay attention in less stimulating situations, where working harder is required to get rewards. Another theory could be that children suffering from attentional problems become captivated by video games as a coping strategy of their behavioral disorder.

Likewise, higher addiction to video gaming was significantly associated with higher LEAF scale and subscales scores (worse cognitive and academic abilities). Correspondingly, Gentile et al. [ 26 ] found that video games addiction among Singaporean adolescents predicted worse school grades and cognitive well-being. In contrast, another study conducted by Regina et al. [ 11 ] demonstrated no evidence that excessive video gaming negatively affects adolescents’ school performances. Our result may be understood in the light of the fact that more time spent on video games means less time spent on academic activities, as well as less hours of sleep overall, which generally make children with heavy video gaming attitudes, less alert and more susceptible to cognitive errors.

In addition to the previous findings, having a father with university education level was significantly associated with lower attention score (better attention) and lower LEAF scale and subscales scores (better cognitive and academic abilities). To our knowledge, no study in the literature discussed the education level of the parents, particularly the father, in this particular context. The cause for our result could be that highly educated fathers raise their children in a similar educative way, which enhances their cognitive and academic skills.

Clinical implications

The current study provides many valuable contributions to the literature. Despite growing numbers of published studies examining cognitive skills in children playing video games, there is a paucity of rigorous ones from which to draw firm conclusions. This study procures firm evidence that addiction to video games is linked to worse memory, attention, as well as cognitive and academic skills in school children. This analysis constitutes a vital first step towards a better understanding of video games addiction in children, supporting its evidence as a plausible entity with detrimental association. By this, the study encourages parents of children addicted to video games to set particular gaming rules (such as limiting gaming time and avoiding exploration of new games), advocates schools to introduce children to other fun activities to do, and highlights the importance of consulting a therapist in extreme cases.

Limitations

There are several limitations that should be noted. First of all, all scales were self-rated, which may only show high risk of addiction to video gaming rather than the diagnosis. Second, residual confounding bias is also possible, since there could be factors related to video gaming such as pre-existing attention problems, ADHD diagnosis that were not measured in this study. Moreover, parents might have over-or underestimated the answers to the questions, possibly predisposing us to information bias. A selection bias is also possible because of the enrollment of the students from three schools only and because of the convenient sample. The group of students chosen in this study were not representative of most of students in their age. Students’ parents filled up the video gaming addiction scale, which could lead to information bias since this scale is not applicable to them. To add on that, since this study is cross-sectional, the findings of this study cannot address the causal relationships among the primary constructs of interest. Finally, the scales used have not been validated in Lebanon. The use of a random sample is an important advantage to the study, thus, the results we obtained can be generalized to the whole population. The results cannot be generalized to the whole population since it only recruited students from private schools, as well as a high percentage of well-educated mothers. A prospective longitudinal study is needed to better follow up the cognitive function of children over time.

In conclusion, the results suggest a correlation between addiction to video games and worse memory, attention, as well as cognitive and academic abilities among school children. Those findings indicate the need for more extensive research, and serve to highlight vital next steps needed in future papers, such as identifying predicting factors that could aid in early detection of video gaming addiction in children.

Availability of data and materials

There is no public access to all data generated or analyzed during this study to preserve the privacy of the identities of the individuals. The dataset that supports the conclusions is available to the corresponding author upon request.

Abbreviations

Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders

11th Revision of International Classification of Diseases

Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder

Game Addiction Scale for Children

Children’s Memory Questionnaire

Learning, Executive and Attention Functioning Scale

Multivariate analysis of covariance

Nielsen website. Multi-plateform gaming: for the win! https://www.nielsen.com/us/en/insights/article/2014/multi-platform-gaming-for-the-win/ .

Jo YS, Bhang SY, Choi JS, Lee HK, Lee SY, Kweon YS. Clinical characteristics of diagnosis for internet gaming disorder: comparison of DSM-5 IGD and ICD-11 GD diagnosis. J Clin Med. 2019;8(7):945.

Article   Google Scholar  

Torres-Rodriguez A, Griffiths MD, Carbonell X, Oberst U. Internet gaming disorder in adolescence: psychological characteristics of a clinical sample. J Behav Addict. 2018;7(3):707–18.

Nogueira M, Faria H, Vitorino A, Silva FG, Serrao NA. Addictive video game use: an emerging pediatric problem? Acta Med Port. 2019;32(3):183–8.

Christakis DA. The challenges of defining and studying “digital addiction” in children. JAMA. 2019;321(23):2277–8.

Kok Eren H, Orsal O. Computer game addiction and loneliness in children. Iran J Public Health. 2018;47(10):1504–10.

PubMed   PubMed Central   Google Scholar  

Choi BY, Huh S, Kim D-J, Suh SW, Lee S-K, Potenza MN. Transitions in problematic internet use: a one-year longitudinal study of boys. Psychiatry Investig. 2019;16(6):433–42.

Du X, Yang Y, Gao P, Qi X, Du G, Zhang Y, et al. Compensatory increase of functional connectivity density in adolescents with internet gaming disorder. Brain Imaging Behav. 2017;11(6):1901–9.

Lim JA, Lee JY, Jung HY, Sohn BK, Choi SW, Kim YJ, et al. Changes of quality of life and cognitive function in individuals with internet gaming disorder: a 6-month follow-up. Medicine. 2016;95(50):e5695.

Teng Z, Nie Q, Guo C, Zhang Q, Liu Y, Bushman BJ. A longitudinal study of link between exposure to violent video games and aggression in Chinese adolescents: the mediating role of moral disengagement. Dev Psychol. 2019;55(1):184–95.

van den Eijnden R, Koning I, Doornwaard S, van Gurp F, Ter Bogt T. The impact of heavy and disordered use of games and social media on adolescents’ psychological, social, and school functioning. J Behav Addict. 2018;7(3):697–706.

Evren C, Evren B, Dalbudak E, Topcu M, Kutlu N. Relationships of internet addiction and Internet gaming disorder symptom severities with probable attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder, aggression and negative affect among university students. Atten Defic Hyperact Disord. 2019;11(4):413–21.

Stavropoulos V, Adams BLM, Beard CL, Dumble E, Trawley S, Gomez R, et al. Associations between attention deficit hyperactivity and internet gaming disorder symptoms: is there consistency across types of symptoms, gender and countries? Addict Behav Rep. 2019;9:100158.

Hawi NS, Samaha M, Griffiths MD. Internet gaming disorder in Lebanon: relationships with age, sleep habits, and academic achievement. J Behav Addict. 2018;7(1):70–8.

Obeid S, Saade S, Haddad C, Sacre H, Khansa W, Al Hajj R, et al. Internet addiction among Lebanese adolescents: the role of self-esteem, anger, depression, anxiety, social anxiety and fear, impulsivity, and aggression—a cross-sectional study. J Nerv Ment Dis. 2019;207(10):838–46.

Hawi NS, Samaha M. Validation of the Arabic version of the internet gaming disorder-20 test. Cyberpsychol Behav Soc Netw. 2017;20(4):268–72.

Hawi NS, Samaha M, Griffiths MD. The digital addiction scale for children: development and validation. Cyberpsychol Behav Soc Netw. 2019;22(12):771–8.

Yılmaz E, Griffiths MD, Kan A. Development and validation of videogame addiction scale for children (VASC). Int J Ment Health Addict. 2017;15(4):869–82.

Drysdale K, Shores A, Levick W. Use of the everyday memory questionnaire with children. Child Neuropsychol. 2004;10(2):67–75.

Edelbrock C, Rancurello M. Childhood hyperactivity: an overview of rating scales and their applications. Clin Psychol Rev. 1985;5(5):429–45.

Castellanos I, Kronenberger WG, Pisoni DB. Questionnaire-based assessment of executive functioning: psychometrics. Appl Neuropsychol Child. 2018;7(2):93–109.

Ozcetin M, Gumustas F, Cag Y, Gokbay IZ, Ozmel A. The relationships between video game experience and cognitive abilities in adolescents. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat. 2019;15:1171–80.

Zhou Z, Li C, Zhu H. An error-related negativity potential investigation of response monitoring function in individuals with internet addiction disorder. Front Behav Neurosci. 2013;7:131.

Song J, Park JH, Han DH, Roh S, Son JH, Choi TY, et al. Comparative study of the effects of bupropion and escitalopram on internet gaming disorder. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2016;70(11):527–35.

Article   CAS   Google Scholar  

Ko CH, Yen JY, Chen CS, Yeh YC, Yen CF. Predictive values of psychiatric symptoms for internet addiction in adolescents: a 2-year prospective study. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2009;163(10):937–43.

Gentile DA, Choo H, Liau A, Sim T, Li D, Fung D, et al. Pathological video game use among youths: a two-year longitudinal study. Pediatrics. 2011;127(2):e319-329.

Download references

Acknowledgements

The authors would like to thank the mothers for accepting to be part of this study.

Author information

Sahar Obeid, Pascale Salameh, and Souheil Hallit are last co-authors

Authors and Affiliations

Faculty of Medicine and Medical Sciences, Holy Spirit University of Kaslik (USEK), Jounieh, Lebanon

Youssef Farchakh & Souheil Hallit

Research and Psychology Departments, Psychiatric Hospital of the Cross, P.O. Box 60096, Jal Eddib, Lebanon

Chadia Haddad & Sahar Obeid

INSERM, Univ. Limoges, CH Esquirol, IRD, U1094 Tropical Neuroepidemiology, Institute of Epidemiology and Tropical Neurology, GEIST, Limoges, France

Chadia Haddad

INSPECT-LB: Institut National de Santé Publique, Epidémiologie Clinique Et Toxicologie-Liban, Beirut, Lebanon

Hala Sacre, Sahar Obeid, Pascale Salameh & Souheil Hallit

Faculty of Arts and Sciences, Holy Spirit University of Kaslik (USEK), Jounieh, Lebanon

Sahar Obeid

Faculty of Pharmacy, Lebanese University, Beirut, Lebanon

Pascale Salameh

Faculty of Medicine, University of Nicosia, Nicosia, Cyprus

You can also search for this author in PubMed   Google Scholar

Contributions

The authors would like to thank Mr. Jihad Gerges, Mr. Joseph Chahine and the following persons of College Central des Moines Libanais Maronites—Jounieh: Father Superior Antoine Salame, General Director Father Dr. Wadih Al Skayem, Mrs. Hélène Chbeir—Director of the kindergarten, Mr. Joseph Rassi—Director of the primary cycle as well as the teachers of the primary cycle for their help in the data collection. The authors would like to thank Pr. William G. Kronenberger for giving us the permission to use the LEAF scale. Special thanks to the parents who participated in this study and for Ms. Marine Arisdakessian for her help in the data entry. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding authors

Correspondence to Sahar Obeid or Souheil Hallit .

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate.

The Psychiatric Hospital of the Cross Ethics and Research Committee approved this study protocol (HPC-012-2018). A written consent was obtained from the students and from their parents prior to starting the data collection.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher's note.

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ . The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver ( http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/ ) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article.

Farchakh, Y., Haddad, C., Sacre, H. et al. Video gaming addiction and its association with memory, attention and learning skills in Lebanese children. Child Adolesc Psychiatry Ment Health 14 , 46 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1186/s13034-020-00353-3

Download citation

Received : 04 June 2020

Accepted : 04 December 2020

Published : 12 December 2020

DOI : https://doi.org/10.1186/s13034-020-00353-3

Share this article

Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content:

Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article.

Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative

  • Video gaming

Child and Adolescent Psychiatry and Mental Health

ISSN: 1753-2000

video game addiction research paper

U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

The .gov means it’s official. Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

The site is secure. The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

  • Publications
  • Account settings

Preview improvements coming to the PMC website in October 2024. Learn More or Try it out now .

  • Advanced Search
  • Journal List
  • Front Psychol

The Association Between Video Gaming and Psychological Functioning

Juliane m. von der heiden.

1 Department of Psychology, Johannes Gutenberg University Mainz, Mainz, Germany

Beate Braun

2 Department of Psychosomatic Medicine, University Medical Center, Mainz, Germany

Kai W. Müller

Boris egloff, associated data.

Video gaming is an extremely popular leisure-time activity with more than two billion users worldwide ( Newzoo, 2017 ). However, the media as well as professionals have underscored the potential dangers of excessive video gaming. With the present research, we aimed to shed light on the relation between video gaming and gamers’ psychological functioning. Questionnaires on personality and psychological health as well as video gaming habits were administered to 2,734 individuals (2,377 male, 357 female, M age = 23.06, SD age = 5.91). Results revealed a medium-sized negative correlation between problematic video gaming and psychological functioning with regard to psychological symptoms, affectivity, coping, and self-esteem. Moreover, gamers’ reasons for playing and their preferred game genres were differentially related to psychological functioning with the most notable findings for distraction-motivated players as well as action game players. Future studies are needed to examine whether these psychological health risks reflect the causes or consequences of video gaming.

Introduction

Video gaming is a very popular leisure activity among adults ( Pew Research Center, 2018 ). The amount of time spent playing video games has increased steadily, from 5.1 h/week in 2011 to 6.5 h/week in 2017 ( The Nielsen Company, 2017 ). Video gaming is known to have some benefits such as improving focus, multitasking, and working memory, but it may also come with costs when it is used heavily. By spending a predominant part of the day gaming, excessive video gamers are at risk of showing lower educational and career attainment, problems with peers, and lower social skills ( Mihara and Higuchi, 2017 ). On the one hand, video game use is widespread, and it may come with certain precursors as well as consequences. On the other hand, little is known about the relations between various video gaming habits and psychological functioning. This study aims to shed light on these important relations using a large sample.

A video game is defined as “a game which we play thanks to an audiovisual apparatus and which can be based on a story” ( Esposito, 2005 ). In the last few years, the amount of scientific research devoted to video game playing has increased (e.g., Ferguson, 2015 ; Calvert et al., 2017 ; Hamari and Keronen, 2017 ). Most scientific studies in this area of research have focused on the extent of video game play and its diverse correlates. While some researchers have emphasized the benefits of game playing and even suggested a therapeutic use of video games ( Primack et al., 2012 ; Granic et al., 2014 ; Colder Carras et al., 2018 ), others have been intrigued by its potential dangers ( Anderson et al., 2010 ; Müller and Wölfling, 2017 ).

Parents and professionals may be worried about their excessively playing children being “addicted.” However, problematic and potentially addictive video game use goes beyond the extent of playing (in hours per week; Skoric et al., 2009 ). It also includes such issues as craving, loss of control, and negative consequences of excessive gaming. While it is still a matter of debate whether problematic video game play should be considered a behavioral addiction , its status as a mental disorder has been clarified since the release of the DSM-5 in 2013. In the DSM-5, the American Psychiatric Association (2013) defined Internet Gaming Disorder with diagnostic criteria closely related to Gambling Disorder. Generally, this decision has been supported by many researchers (e.g., Petry et al., 2014 ) but has also caused controversies. Researchers have criticized the selection of diagnostic criteria and the vague definition of the Internet Gaming Disorder construct, which excludes offline games from being related to addictive use (e.g., Griffiths et al., 2016 ; Bean et al., 2017 ).

Several studies, literature reviews, and meta-analyses have focused on the correlates of problematic video gaming, usually assessed as a continuum with addiction marking the upper end of the scale (e.g., Ferguson et al., 2011 ; Kuss and Griffiths, 2012 ). The degree of addictive video game use has been found to be related to personality traits such as low self-esteem ( Ko et al., 2005 ) and low self-efficacy ( Jeong and Kim, 2011 ), anxiety, and aggression ( Mehroof and Griffiths, 2010 ), and even to clinical symptoms of depression and anxiety disorders ( Wang et al., 2018 ). Potential consequences of video game use have been identified as well, such as a lack of real-life friends ( Kowert et al., 2014a ), stress and maladaptive coping ( Milani et al., 2018 ), lower psychosocial well-being and loneliness ( Lemmens et al., 2011 ), psychosomatic problems ( Müller et al., 2015 ; Milani et al., 2018 ), and decreased academic achievement ( Chiu et al., 2004 ; Gentile, 2009 ). Effect sizes have varied widely across studies ( Ferguson et al., 2011 ). There seem to be sex and age differences with regard to video gaming behavior: potentially problematic video gaming was found to be more likely among males than females (e.g., Greenberg et al., 2010 ; Estévez et al., 2017 ), and among younger gamers ( Rehbein et al., 2016 ).

In addition to looking at problematic video game use and its relation to psychological functioning, it is relevant to also focus on why individuals play video games. Players use video games for very different reasons ( Ryan et al., 2006 ; Yee, 2006 ) such as to distract themselves from daily hassles or because they enjoy the social relationships they have developed in the virtual world. Potentially problematic video gaming has been found to be related to various reasons for playing such as coping and escape ( Hussain and Griffiths, 2009 ; Schneider et al., 2018 ), socialization ( Laconi et al., 2017 ), and personal satisfaction ( Ng and Wiemer-Hastings, 2005 ). Coping ( Laconi et al., 2017 ), social interaction, and competition were among the main reasons for gaming among males but not among females ( Lucas and Sherry, 2004 ). Mixed results emerged concerning age differences ( Greenberg et al., 2010 ), but especially younger gamers seemed to be motivated for video gaming by social interactions ( Hilgard et al., 2013 ). However, so far it remains unclear to what extent people’s various reasons for playing video games are differentially related to their psychological functioning.

Besides investigating the links between potentially problematic video game use and psychological functioning as well as between reasons for playing video games and psychological functioning, it is relevant to also look at which game genres individuals prefer. Correlates of preferences for certain game genres (e.g., simulation, strategy, action, role-playing) are cognitive enhancement ( Dobrowolski et al., 2015 ; Bediou et al., 2018 ), but also the amount of time spent playing ( Lemmens and Hendriks, 2016 ; Rehbein et al., 2016 ) and psychopathological symptoms ( Laconi et al., 2017 ). Males were shown to prefer action and strategy games, whereas females showed a preference for games of skill ( Scharkow et al., 2015 ; Rehbein et al., 2016 ). Younger gamers seemed to prefer action games, older players more so games of skill ( Scharkow et al., 2015 ). However, it is not yet understood to what extent preferences for certain video game genres are differentially related to psychological functioning.

Typically, research has focused merely on violent video games (e.g., Anderson and Bushman, 2001 ; Elson and Ferguson, 2014 ) or one specific game within one specific game genre (frequently World of Warcraft; Graham and Gosling, 2013 ; Visser et al., 2013 ; Herodotou et al., 2014 ), thereby neglecting the variety of possible gaming habits across various game genres.

In the present study, our objective was to examine the relation between video gaming and psychological functioning in a fine-grained manner. For this purpose, we examined psychological functioning by employing various variables such as psychological symptoms, coping strategies, and social support. Likewise, we assessed video gaming in a similarly detailed way, ranging from (a) problematic video game use, (b) the reasons for playing, to (c) the preferred game genres. This strategy prevented us from making potentially invalid generalizations about video gaming in general and allowed us to examine the spectrum of gaming habits and the respective relations between such habits and a diverse set of variables representing psychological functioning.

Playing video games excessively should be appealing to individuals with poor psychological functioning because games allow people to avoid their everyday problems and instead immerse themselves in another environment ( Taquet et al., 2017 ). Moreover, video games offer people a chance to connect with other people socially despite any more or less evident psychological problems they may have ( Kowert et al., 2014b ; Mazurek et al., 2015 ). On the other hand, potentially problematic video game use may also lead to psychological problems because it reduces the amount of time and the number of opportunities gamers have to practice real-life behavior ( Gentile, 2009 ). Thus, we expected to find a negative correlation between problematic video gaming and variables representing psychological functioning such that we expected more potentially problematic video game use to be related to dysfunctional coping strategies ( Wood and Griffith, 2007 ), negative affectivity ( Mathiak et al., 2011 ), and poor school performance ( Mihara and Higuchi, 2017 ). Moreover, we expected to find differential correlates of people’s reasons for playing video games and their psychological functioning: Playing for escape-oriented reasons such as distraction should go along with diverse indices of poor psychological functioning ( Király et al., 2015 ), whereas playing for gain-oriented reasons such as the storyline or the social connections in the game should be related to adequate psychological functioning ( Longman et al., 2009 ). Also, we expected to find people’s preferred game genres (e.g., strategy, action) to be differentially related to their psychological functioning ( Park et al., 2016 ). Finally, we aimed to shed light on the unique contribution of each measure of psychological functioning to the prediction of problematic video game use.

Materials and Methods

Participants 1.

A total of N = 2,891 individuals (2,421 male, 470 female) with a mean age of 23.17 years ( SD = 5.99, Range: 13–65) participated in our study. Of these participants, N = 2,734 (95%) confirmed their use of video games and were thus included in further analyses (2,377 male, 357 female, with a mean age of 23.06 years; SD = 5.91, Range: 13–65). The distribution of participants with regard to sex and age mirrors the findings of past research with males and younger individuals being more likely to play video games (e.g., Griffiths et al., 2004 ). Participants’ place of residence was Germany.

Procedure and Instruments 2

We posted links to our online questionnaire on various online forums as well as on popular online game sites. To achieve heterogeneity of the sample, no exclusion criteria other than having access to the Internet and understanding German were specified. As an incentive to participate in the study, four vouchers of 50€ were raffled.

Video Gaming

Potentially problematic video game use.

The AICA-S, the Scale for the Assessment of Internet and Computer game Addiction ( Wölfling et al., 2016 ), was used to assess participants’ gaming behavior with regard to potential problematic use. Based on the DSM criteria for Internet Gaming Disorder (tolerance, craving, loss of control, emotion regulation, withdrawal, and unsuccessful attempts to cut back), this standardized self-report scale consists of 15 items usually with a five-point scale ranging from 1 ( never ) to 5 ( very often ). The final score (Min = 0, Max = 27 points) is computed using weighted scoring (items with an item-total correlation > 0.55 in the norm sample are weighted double; Wölfling et al., 2011 ). The AICA-S score can be used to differentiate between regular (0–6.5 points) and problematic use of video games (7–13 points: abuse; 13.5–27 points: addiction). In our sample, N = 2,265 (83%) were identified as regular gamers, and N = 469 (17%) as problematic gamers. We used the AICA-S as a continuous variable for all further analyses ( M = 3.98, SD = 3.22, Range: 0–24). The instrument has been validated for different age groups in the general population and in clinical samples ( Müller et al., 2014a , 2019 , but note small sample size; Müller et al., 2014b ). Cronbach’s alpha was α = 0.70. As expected, the AICA-S score was correlated with male sex ( r = 0.17 ∗∗∗ ) and age ( r = –0.15 ∗∗∗ ). On average, participants played video games for M = 4.09 hours per weekday ( SD = 4.44, Range: 0–24), and M = 4.21 h per day at the weekend ( SD = 2.99, Range: 0–24).

Reasons for playing

Gamers indicated how often they played video games for certain reasons. They rated each of 10 reasons separately on Likert scales ranging from 1 ( never ) to 4 ( very often ). The most prevalent reasons were relaxation ( M = 2.96, SD = 0.91), amusement ( M = 2.94, SD = 0.85), and because of the storyline ( M = 2.67, SD = 1.10).

Game genres

Gamers were asked how often they usually played various video game subgenres such as first-person shooter, round-based strategy, massively multiplayer online role-playing games (MMORPGs), life simulations, and others. Ratings were made on Likert scales ranging from 1 ( never ) to 4 ( very often ). Using Apperley’s (2006) classification of game genres, we categorized the subgenres into the main genres action ( M = 2.54, SD = 0.84), strategy ( M = 2.13, SD = 0.80), role-playing ( M = 2.01, SD = 0.73), and simulation ( M = 1.58, SD = 0.44). A cluster for unclassified subgenres ( M = 1.54, SD = 0.39) was added to additionally account for such subgenres as jump’n’runs and games of skill. Descriptive statistics and intercorrelations for all measures (including sex and age) are presented in Supplementary Tables S1–S4 .

Psychological Functioning

Participants provided ratings of their psychological functioning on the following constructs:

General psychopathology

The SCL-K-9 ( Klaghofer and Brähler, 2001 ), a short version of the SCL-90-R ( Derogatis, 1975 ), was administered to assess participants’ subjective impairment regarding psychological symptoms (somatization, obsessive-compulsive, interpersonal sensitivity, depression, anxiety, hostility, phobic anxiety, paranoid ideation, and psychoticism). The SCL-K-9 score is strongly correlated with the original score of the SCL-90-R ( r = 0.93). The 9 items were answered on 5-point Likert-type scales ranging from 1 ( do not agree at all ) to 5 ( agree completely ). Cronbach’s alpha was satisfactory (α = 0.77).

We assessed 10 coping strategies with the Brief COPE ( Carver, 1997 ; German version by Knoll et al., 2005 ), which is the shorter version of the COPE ( Carver et al., 1989 ): self-distraction, denial, substance use, venting, self-blame, behavioral disengagement, acceptance, active coping, planning, and positive reframing. The two items per subscale were administered on 5-point Likert-type scales ranging from 1 ( never ) to 5 ( very often ). Intercorrelations of the two items per subscale ranged from r = 0.32, p < 0.001 for positive reframing to r = 0.78, p < 0.001 for substance use (with one exception: r = -0.05, p = 0.01 for self-distraction).

We measured general affect as a trait and affect during video gaming as a state using the German version ( Krohne et al., 1996 ) of the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS; Watson et al., 1988 ). On a 5-point Likert-type scale ranging from 1 ( not at all ) to 5 ( completely ), participants rated the intensity of 20 adjectives. Cronbach’s alpha was α = 0.78 for general positive affect, α = 0.83 for general negative affect, α = 0.85 for positive affect while playing, and α = 0.83 for negative affect while playing.

The measure for the assessment of shyness in adults ( Asendorpf, 1997 ) consists of 5 items that were answered on a 5-point Likert-type scale ranging from 1 ( not at all ) to 5 ( completely ). Cronbach’s alpha was excellent (α = 0.86).

We administered the German version ( Elbing, 1991 ) of the NYU Loneliness Scale ( Rubenstein and Shaver, 1982 ). The 4 items were answered on 5- to 6-point Likert-type scales. Cronbach’s alpha was satisfactory (α = 0.79).

Preference for solitude

A 10-item measure of preference for solitude ( Nestler et al., 2011 ) was answered on a 6-point Likert-type scale ranging from 1 ( not at all ) to 6 ( completely ). Cronbach’s alpha was excellent (α = 0.86).

Life satisfaction

Participants answered a one-item life satisfaction measure on a 4-point Likert-type scale ranging from 1 ( not at all ) to 4 ( completely ).

Self-esteem

We administered the German version ( von Collani and Herzberg, 2003 ) of the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale (RSES; Rosenberg, 1979 ). The 10 items were answered on a 4-point Likert-type scale ranging from 1 ( not at all ) to 4 ( completely ). Cronbach’s alpha was excellent (α = 0.88).

Self-efficacy

We administered a 10-item generalized self-efficacy scale ( Schwarzer and Jerusalem, 1995 ), which was answered on a 4-point Likert-type scale ranging from 1 ( not at all ) to 4 ( completely ). Cronbach’s alpha was excellent (α = 0.86).

Social support and friends

We administered the perceived available social support subscale from the Berlin Social Support Scales (BSSS; Schwarzer and Schulz, 2003 ). The 8 items were answered on a 5-point Likert-type scale ranging from 1 ( not at all ) to 5 ( completely ). Cronbach’s alpha was excellent (α = 0.94). Participants indicated how many offline friends and offline acquaintances they had ( r = 0.44, p < 0.001) as well as how many online friends and online acquaintances they had ( r = 0.33, p < 0.001). Due to left-skewed distributions, we logarithmized the data before aggregation.

Participants reported their grade point average. German grades are assessed on a scale that ranges from 1 ( excellent ) to 6 ( insufficient ). Thus, higher scores indicate worse grades.

Participants further reported their sex and age. Both were used as control variables in further analyses.

In a first step, we computed zero-order correlations between the video gaming variables and the measures of psychological functioning. In a second step, we computed partial correlations in which we controlled for sex and age because past research has repeatedly shown that sex and age are correlated with both video gaming ( Homer et al., 2012 ; Mihara and Higuchi, 2017 ) and psychological functioning ( Kessler et al., 2007 ; Nolen-Hoeksema, 2012 ). Finally, we explored the unique contribution of each measure of psychological functioning to the prediction of potentially problematic video gaming. Therefore, we computed regressions with potentially problematic video gaming as the dependent variable and sex, age, and the measures of psychological functioning as predictors (entered simultaneously into the regression equation). By employing this procedure, we were able to determine the effect that each variable had over and above the other ones. For instance, we could identify whether general psychopathology was predictive of potentially problematic video game use when the influence of all other variables (e.g., shyness, loneliness, and others) was held constant.

Additionally, we included analyses regarding sex and age differences in the link between video gaming and psychological functioning. Since we collected a self-selected sample where different sexes and age groups were not represented equally, our findings are only preliminary, but may stimulate future research.

Potentially Problematic Video Game Use and Psychological Functioning

First, we examined whether potentially problematic video game use was related to various psychological functioning variables. As can be seen in Table 1 , the results for the zero-order correlations were similar to those for the partial correlations in which we controlled for sex and age. A medium-sized positive relation to the potentially problematic use of video games emerged for the presence of psychological symptoms including depression, anxiety, and hostility. Furthermore, several coping strategies were differentially associated with the potentially problematic use of video games: Self-blame and behavioral disengagement showed the strongest positive relations to potentially problematic video game use, followed by denial, acceptance, substance use, self-distraction, and venting. Planning, active coping, and, to a lesser extent, positive reframing were negatively associated with the potentially problematic use of video games. Moreover, the association with potentially problematic video game use was negative for general positive affect and positive and larger in size for general negative affect. However, potentially problematic video game use was clearly positively associated with the experience of both positive and negative affect while playing. Further, a preference for solitude, shyness, and loneliness were positively correlated with the potentially problematic use of video games. Lower self-esteem, lower life satisfaction, and, to a lesser extent, poorer perceived social support and lower self-efficacy went along with potentially problematic video game use. There was an association between fewer offline friends and acquaintances but more online connections with potentially problematic video gaming. Finally, poorer performance in school (i.e., higher grades) was related to the potentially problematic use of video games. These results suggest that potentially problematic video gaming goes along with poor psychological functioning and vice versa.

Associations between potentially problematic video gaming and psychological functioning.

Reasons for Playing Video Games and Psychological Functioning

Second, we investigated whether players’ reasons for playing video games were differentially related to the psychological functioning variables. Table 2 presents the partial correlations, controlling for sex and age. Using video games to distract oneself from stress was clearly connected to a high level of psychological symptoms. Distraction-motivated gamers preferred coping strategies such as self-blame, behavioral disengagement, self-distraction, denial, substance use, venting, and acceptance, but they neglected active coping and planning. They showed less general positive affect and more negative affect both in general and while playing as well as more positive affect while playing. These gamers further reported low self-esteem and low life satisfaction, loneliness, a preference for solitude, shyness, a lack of self-efficacy and social support, and poor achievement in school. A similar but somewhat less extreme picture was revealed for gamers who played video games in order to have something to talk about . However, these gamers reported more online connections. Gamers who played video games to improve their real-life abilities also reported more online connections. In addition, these gamers showed higher levels of general positive affect. The strongest association with online friends and acquaintances emerged, as expected, for gamers who played because of the social relations in the virtual world. Although all reasons for playing video games were related to positive affect while playing, the strongest associations emerged for gamers who played because of the social relations , to stimulate their imagination , and for curiosity . It is interesting that, for gamers who played video games because of the storyline and for relaxation , there was a relation only to positive but not to negative affect while playing. Reasons for playing were only weakly related to sex and age (see Supplementary Table S2 ). In sum, several reasons for playing video games were differentially associated with psychological functioning.

Associations between reasons for playing video games and psychological functioning.

Video Game Genre and Psychological Functioning

Third, we examined whether players’ preferences for different video game genres were differentially associated with the measures of psychological functioning. Table 3 shows the partial correlations in which we controlled for sex and age. There was a weak connection between general psychological symptoms and all of the video game genres we investigated except strategy. A preference for action games had the strongest association with affect while playing. Thus, action games seem to be both rewarding and a source of frustration. A preference for action games went along with poorer school performance. Gamers who preferred role-playing games scored higher on shyness and a preference for solitude and lower on self-esteem; they also reported fewer offline connections. By contrast, preferences for games of the unclassified category on average went along with a larger number of offline friends and more positive affect, both while playing and in general. Two game genres (i.e., role-playing and unclassified games) were related to the coping strategy of self-distraction. Because preferred game genre was related to participants’ sex (see Supplementary Table S3 ), we had a more detailed look at the correlations between preferred game genre and psychological functioning separately for both sexes: For males ( n = 2,377), the strongest correlation between general psychopathology and game genre emerged for action ( r = 0.08, p < 0.001), followed by role playing ( r = 0.07, p < 0.01), and unclassified ( r = 0.07, p < 0.01). For females ( n = 357), the strongest relation between general psychopathology and game genre emerged for simulation ( r = 0.17, p < 0.01). Differences were also found regarding the strength of the relation between number of friends online and the genre action: r = 0.06, p < 0.01 for males, and r = 0.27, p < 0.001 for females. Similarly, preferred game genre was related to participants’ age (see Supplementary Table S3 ). However, there were merely differences with regard to the relation of psychological functioning and game genre, when analyzed separately for different age groups (<19 years, n = 557; 19–30 years, n = 1916; >31 years, n = 261). In sum, our results speak to the idea that individuals with different levels of psychological functioning differ in their choices of game genres and vice versa.

Associations between preferred video game genre and psychological functioning.

Predicting Potentially Problematic Video Game Use by Psychological Functioning Variables

In a final step, we entered all of the investigated psychological functioning variables as well as sex and age as predictors of the potentially problematic use of video games. By employing this procedure, we were able to determine the unique contribution of each psychological functioning variable when the influence of all other variables was held constant. As Table 4 shows, the number of online friends and acquaintances as well as positive affect while playing were most predictive of potentially problematic video game use over and above all other variables. General psychopathology, a lack of offline connections, and poor school performance were weaker but still relevant predictors of potentially problematic video game use.

Prediction of potentially problematic video game use by psychological functioning variables.

With this study, we aimed to shed light on the association of diverse video gaming habits with gamers’ psychological functioning. Drawing on a large sample, our results revealed a medium-sized relation between potentially problematic video game use and poor psychological functioning with regard to general psychological symptoms, maladaptive coping strategies, negative affectivity, low self-esteem, and a preference for solitude as well as poor school performance. These findings are in line with those of prior work (e.g., Kuss and Griffiths, 2012 ; Milani et al., 2018 ). Also, reasons for playing video games were differentially related to psychological functioning with the most pronounced findings for escape-oriented in contrast to gain-oriented motives. Specifically, distraction-motivated gaming went along with higher symptom ratings, lower self-esteem, and more negative affectivity, whereas playing to establish social relationships in the virtual world was related to a larger number of online connections and more positive affect while playing. Furthermore, there were only weak relations between the preferred game genres and psychological functioning. The action games genre was associated with the strongest ratings of affect while playing. These results on reasons and genres may help to explain conflicting findings of former studies, because in our work we examined various reasons for playing, several game genres, and various aspects of psychological functioning simultaneously. Finally, positive affect while playing and a larger number of online friends were the strongest unique predictors of potentially problematic video game use, followed by psychological symptoms, a lack of offline connections, and poor school performance. These findings suggest that, on the one hand, independent of one’s psychological conditions, enjoying oneself during gaming (i.e., experiencing positive affect, connecting with online friends) may go along with potentially problematic use of video games. On the other hand, poor psychological functioning seems to be a unique risk factor for potentially problematic video gaming.

The presented results are generally in line with previous work that has identified a connection between video gaming and psychological health, academic problems, and social problems ( Ferguson et al., 2011 ; Müller et al., 2015 ). However, our study moved beyond prior research by providing in-depth analyses of both video gaming habits (including potentially problematic use, reasons for playing, and preferred game genre) and psychological functioning (including psychological symptoms, coping styles, affectivity, as well as variables that are related to individuals and their social environments). In addition, we identified unique predictors of potentially problematic video game use.

How can the findings on differential relations between video gaming and various indices of psychological functioning – ranging from beneficial results ( Latham et al., 2013 ) to unfavorable results ( Barlett et al., 2009 ; Möller and Krahé, 2009 ; Anderson et al., 2010 ) – be integrated? According to Kanfer and Phillips (1970) , problematic behavior (e.g., excessive video gaming) can be understood as a function of the situation (e.g., being rejected by a peer); the organism (e.g., low self-esteem); the person’s thoughts, physical reactions, and feelings (e.g., sadness, anger); and finally, the short- as well as long-term consequences of the behavior (termed SORKC model). In the short run, according to our results, playing video games may be a way to distract oneself from everyday hassles and may lead to positive affect while playing and a feeling of being connected to like-minded people, all of which are factors that have an immediate reinforcing value. In the long run, however, spending many hours per day in front of a computer screen may prevent a person from (a) developing and practicing functional coping strategies, (b) finding friends and support in the social environment, and (c) showing proper school achievement, factors that are potentially harmful to the person. Thus, differentiating between short- and long-term perspectives may help us understanding the differential correlates of intensive video gaming.

When is it appropriate to speak of video game addiction? More and more researchers have suggested a continuum between engagement ( Charlton and Danforth, 2007 ; Skoric et al., 2009 ) and pathological gaming/addiction, instead of a categorical perspective. In part, this recommendation has also been followed in the DSM-5 ( American Psychiatric Association, 2013 ) where Internet Gaming Disorder is classified with different degrees of severity, ranging from mild to moderate to severe, according to the functional impairment associated with it. The AICA-S also allows for a differential perspective on gaming behavior by providing ways to assess both the time spent playing video games and the main DSM criteria that indicate Internet Gaming Disorder. However, in our study we did not aim at making a diagnosis, but at having a closer look at potentially problematic gaming behavior and its correlates in a non-clinical sample.

In sum, it seems relevant to assess not only the extent of video game use but also the reasons behind this behavior (e.g., distraction) and the concrete rewards that come from playing (e.g., the experience of strong affect while playing action games) to fully understand the relation between video gaming and psychological functioning.

Limitations and Future Directions

With the present study, we aimed to uncover the association between video gaming and psychological functioning. Our approach was cross-sectional and warrants interpretative caution because correlations cannot determine the direction of causation. It remains unclear whether potentially problematic gaming is a factor that contributes to the development of psychological dysfunction or whether psychological dysfunction contributes to potentially problematic gaming. Also, a third factor (e.g., preexisting mental difficulties) may produce both psychological dysfunction and potentially problematic gaming. Thus, longitudinal studies that are designed to identify the causal pathway may provide a promising avenue for future research. Future studies may also answer the question whether the link between video gaming and psychological functioning is moderated by sex, age, the reasons for playing, or the preferred game genre. In addition, it is important not to forget that the present results are based on a self-selected sample in which potentially problematic video gamers were overrepresented (e.g., Festl et al., 2013 , for a representative sample). Thus, future research should replicate our findings in a representative sample. Further, we relied on self-reported data, which is a plausible method for assessing inner affairs such as people’s reasons for their behaviors, but it would be helpful to back up our findings with evidence derived from sources such as peers, caregivers, and health specialists. Our work reflects only a first approach to the topic, and future work may additionally collect in-game behavioral data from the players ( McCreery et al., 2012 ; Billieux et al., 2013 ) to objectively and more specifically investigate diverse patterns of use. Furthermore, one must not forget that the used taxonomy to classify video game genres is only one of various possible options and one should “think of each individual game as belonging to several genres at once” ( Apperley, 2006 , p. 19). Finally, some of the effects reported in our paper were rather modest in size. This is not surprising considering the complexity and multiple determinants of human behavior. In our analyses, we thoroughly controlled for the influence of sex and age and still found evidence that video gaming was differentially related to measures of psychological functioning.

The current study adds to the knowledge on gaming by uncovering the specific relations between video gaming and distinct measures of psychological functioning. Potentially problematic video gaming was found to be associated with positive affect and social relationships while playing but also with psychological symptoms, maladaptive coping strategies, negative affectivity, low self-esteem, a preference for solitude, and poor school performance. Including gamers’ reasons for playing video games and their preferred game genres helped deepen the understanding of the specific and differential associations between video gaming and psychological health. This knowledge might help developing adequate interventions that are applied prior to the occurrence of psychological impairments that may go along with potentially problematic video gaming.

Ethics Statement

In our online survey, participants were given information on voluntary participation, risks, confidentiality/anonymity, and right to withdraw. Whilst participants were not signing a separate consent form, consent was obtained by virtue of completion. We implemented agreed procedures to maintain the confidentiality of participant data.

Author Contributions

BB, BE, JH, and KM conceived and designed the study. BB, JH, and KM collected and prepared the data. JH analyzed the data. BE and JH wrote the manuscript.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

1 The data were gathered as part of a larger project ( Stopfer et al., 2015 ; Braun et al., 2016 ). However, the analyses in the present article do not overlap with analyses from previous work.

2 Other measures were administered, but they were not relevant to the present research questions and are thus not mentioned in this paper. The data set and analysis script supporting the conclusions of this manuscript can be retrieved from https://osf.io/emrpw/?view_only=856491775efe4f99b407e258c2f2fa8d .

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01731/full#supplementary-material

  • American Psychiatric Association (2013). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders: DSM-5 , 5th Edn Washington DC: American Psychiatric Association. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Anderson C. A., Bushman B. J. (2001). Effects of violent video games on aggressive behavior, aggressive cognition, aggressive affect, physiological arousal, and prosocial behavior: a meta-analytic review of the scientific literature. Psychol. Sci. 12 353–359. 10.1111/1467-9280.00366 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Anderson C. A., Shibuya A., Ihori N., Swing E. L., Bushman B. J., Sakamoto A., et al. (2010). Violent video game effects on aggression, empathy, and prosocial behavior in eastern and western countries: a meta-analytic review. Psychol. Bull. 136 151–173. 10.1037/a0018251 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Apperley T. H. (2006). Genre and game studies: toward a critical approach to video game genres. Simul. Gaming 37 6–23. 10.1177/1046878105282278 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Asendorpf J. B. (1997). SGSE-Schüchternheits- und Geselligkeitsskalen für Erwachsene [Scales on shyness and sociality for adults ] . Berlin: Humboldt-Universität. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Barlett C. P., Anderson C. A., Swing E. L. (2009). Video game effects—Confirmed, suspected, and speculative: a review of the evidence. Simul. Gaming 40 377–403. 10.1177/1046878108327539 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bean A. M., Nielsen R. K. L., van Rooij A. J., Ferguson C. J. (2017). Video game addiction: the push to pathologize video games. Prof. Psychol. Res. Pr. 48 378–389. 10.1037/pro0000150 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bediou B., Adams D. M., Mayer R. E., Tipton E., Green C. S., Bavelier D. (2018). Meta-analysis of action video game impact on perceptual, attentional, and cognitive skills. Psychol. Bull. 144 77–110. 10.1037/bul0000130 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Billieux J., Van der Linden M., Achab S., Khazaal Y., Paraskevopoulos L., Zullino D., et al. (2013). Why do you play World of Warcraft? An in-depth exploration of self-reported motivations to play online and in-game behaviours in the virtual world of Azeroth. Comput. Hum. Behav. 29 103–109. 10.1016/j.chb.2012.07.021 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Braun B., Stopfer J. M., Müller K. W., Beutel M. E., Egloff B. (2016). Personality and video gaming: comparing regular gamers, non-gamers, and gaming addicts and differentiating between game genres. Comput. Hum. Behav. 55 406–412. 10.1016/j.chb.2015.09.041 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Calvert S. L., Appelbaum M., Dodge K. A., Graham S., Nagayama Hall G. C., Hamby S., et al. (2017). The american psychological association task force assessment of violent video games: science in the service of public interest. Am. Psychol. 72 126–143. 10.1037/a0040413 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Carver C. S. (1997). You want to measure coping but your protocol’s too long: consider the brief cope. Int. J. Behav. Med. 4 92–100. 10.1207/s15327558ijbm0401_6 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Carver C. S., Scheier M. F., Weintraub J. K. (1989). Assessing coping strategies: a theoretically based approach. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 56 267–283. 10.1037/0022-3514.56.2.267 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Charlton J. P., Danforth I. D. (2007). Distinguishing addiction and high engagement in the context of online game playing. Comput. Hum. Behav. 23 1531–1548. 10.1016/j.chb.2005.07.002 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Chiu S. I., Lee J. Z., Huang D. H. (2004). Video game addiction in children and teenagers in Taiwan. Cyberpsychol. Behav. 7 571–581. 10.1089/cpb.2004.7.571 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Colder Carras M., Van Rooij A. J., Spruijit-Metz D., Kvedar J., Griffiths M. D., Carabas Y., et al. (2018). Commercial video games as therapy: a new research agenda to unlock the potential of a global pastime. Front. Psychiatry 8 : 300 . 10.3389/fpsyt.2017.00300 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Derogatis L. R. (1975). SCL-90-R: Symptom Checklist-90-R: Administration, Scoring, and Procedures Manual. London: NCS Pearson. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Dobrowolski P., Hanusz K., Sobczyk B., Skorko M., Wiatrow A. (2015). Cognitive enhancement in video game players: the role of video game genre. Comput. Hum. Behav. 44 59–63. 10.1016/j.chb.2014.11.051 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Elbing E. (1991). Einsamkeit: Psychologische Konzepte, Forschungsbefunde und Treatmentansätze [Loneliness: Psychological Concepts, Research Findings, and Treatments]. Göttingen: Hogrefe. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Elson M., Ferguson C. J. (2014). Twenty-five years of research on violence in digital games and aggression: empirical evidence, perspectives, and a debate gone astray. Eur. Psychol. 19 33–46. 10.1027/1016-9040/a000147 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Esposito N. (2005). “ A short and simple definition of what a videogame is ,” in Proceedings of the DiGRA (Digital Games Research Association) Conference: Changing Views-Worlds in Play , (British Columbia: University of Vancouver; ). [ Google Scholar ]
  • Estévez A., Jáuregui P., Sánchez-Marcos I., López-González H., Griffiths M. D. (2017). Attachment and emotion regulation in substance addictions and behavioral addictions. J. Behav. Addict. 6 534–544. 10.1556/2006.6.2017.086 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Ferguson C. J. (2015). Do angry birds make for angry children? A meta-analysis of video game influences on children’s and adolescents’ aggression, mental health, prosocial behavior, and academic performance. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 10 646–666. 10.1177/1745691615592234 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Ferguson C. J., Coulson M., Barnett J. (2011). A meta-analysis of pathological gaming prevalence and comorbidity with mental health, academic and social problems. J. Psychiatr. Res. 45 1573–1578. 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2011.09.005 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Festl R., Scharkow M., Quandt T. (2013). Problematic computer game use among adolescents, younger and older adults. Addiction 108 592–599. 10.1111/add.12016 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Gentile D. (2009). Pathological video-game use among youth ages 8 to 18: a national study. Psychol. Sci. 20 594–602. 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02340.x [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Graham L. T., Gosling S. D. (2013). Personality profiles associated with different motivations for playing World of Warcraft. Cyberpsychol. Behav. Soc. Netw. 16 189–193. 10.1089/cyber.2012.0090 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Granic I., Lobel A., Engels R. C. (2014). The benefits of playing video games. Am. Psychol. 69 66–78. 10.1037/a0034857 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Greenberg B. S., Sherry J., Lachlan K., Lucas K., Holmstrom A. (2010). Orientations to video games among gender and age groups. Simul. Gaming 41 238–259. 10.1177/1046878108319930 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Griffiths M., Van Rooij A. J., Kardefeldt-Winther D., Starcevic V., Király O., Pallesen S., et al. (2016). Working towards an international consensus on criteria for assessing internet gaming disorder: a critical commentary on Petry et al. (2014). Addiction 111 167–175. 10.1111/add.13057 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Griffiths M. D., Davies M. N., Chappell D. (2004). Demographic factors and playing variables in online computer gaming. CyberPsychol. Behav. 7 479–487. 10.1089/cpb.2004.7.479 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hamari J., Keronen L. (2017). Why do people play games? A meta-analysis. Int. J. Inform. Manag. 37 125–141. 10.1016/j.ijinfomgt.2017.01.006 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Herodotou C., Kambouri M., Winters N. (2014). Dispelling the myth of the socio-emotionally dissatisfied gamer. Comput. Hum. Behav. 32 23–31. 10.1016/j.chb.2013.10.054 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hilgard J., Engelhardt C. R., Bartholow B. D. (2013). Individual differences in motives, preferences, and pathology in video games: the gaming attitudes, motives, and experiences scales (GAMES). Front. Psychol. 4 : 608 . 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00608 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Homer B. D., Hayward E. O., Frye J., Plass J. L. (2012). Gender and player characteristics in video game play of preadolescents. Comput. Hum. Behav. 28 1782–1789. 10.1016/j.chb.2012.04.018 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hussain Z., Griffiths M. D. (2009). Excessive use of massively multi-player online role-playing games: a pilot study. Int. J. Mental Health Addict. 7 : 563 10.1007/s11469-009-9202-8 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Jeong E. J., Kim D. H. (2011). Social activities, self-efficacy, game attitudes, and game addiction. Cyberpsychol. Behav. Soc. Netw. 14 213–221. 10.1089/cyber.2009.0289 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kanfer F. H., Phillips J. (1970). Learning Foundations of Behavior Therapy. New York, NY: Wiley. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kessler R. C., Amminger G. P., Aguilar-Gaxiola S., Alonso J., Lee S., Ustun T. B. (2007). Age of onset of mental disorders: a review of recent literature. Curr. Opin. Psychiatry 20 359–364. 10.1097/YCO.0b013e32816ebc8c [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Király O., Urbán R., Griffith M. D., Ágoston C., Nagygyörgy K., Kökönyei G., et al. (2015). The mediating effect of gaming motivation between psychiatric symptoms and problematic online gaming: an online survey. J. Med. Int. Res. 17 : e88 . 10.2196/jmir.3515 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Klaghofer R., Brähler E. (2001). Konstruktion und teststatistische Prüfung einer Kurzform der SCL-90-R [Construction and statistical evaluation of a short version of the SCL-90-R]. Z. Klin. Psychol. Psychiatr. Psychother. 49 115–124. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Knoll N., Rieckmann N., Schwarzer R. (2005). Coping as a mediator between personality and stress outcomes: a longitudinal study with cataract surgery patients. Eur. J. Personal. 19 229–247. 10.1002/per.546 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Ko C. H., Yen J. Y., Chen C. C., Chen S. H., Yen C. F. (2005). Gender differences and related factors affecting online gaming addiction among Taiwanese adolescents. J. Nervous Mental Dis. 193 273–277. 10.1097/01.nmd.0000158373.85150.57 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kowert R., Domahidi E., Festl R., Quandt T. (2014a). Social gaming, lonely life? The impact of digital game play on adolescents’ social circles. Comput. Hum. Behav. 36 385–390. 10.1016/j.chb.2014.04.003 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kowert R., Domahidi E., Quandt T. (2014b). The relationship between online video game involvement and gaming-related friendships among emotionally sensitive individuals. Cyberpsychol. Behav. Soc. Netw. 17 447–453. 10.1089/cyber.2013.0656 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Krohne H. W., Egloff B., Kohlmann C. W., Tausch A. (1996). Untersuchungen mit einer deutschen version der positive and negative affect schedule (PANAS) [Investigations with a German version of the PANAS]. Diagnostica 42 139–156. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kuss D. J., Griffiths M. D. (2012). Internet gaming addiction: a systematic review of empirical research. Int. J. Mental Health Addict. 10 278–296. 10.1007/s11469-011-9318-5 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Laconi S., Pirès S., Chabrol H. (2017). Internet gaming disorder, motives, game genres, and psychopathology. Comput. Hum. Behav. 75 652–659. 10.1016/j.chb.2017.06.012 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Latham A. J., Patston L. L., Tippett L. J. (2013). The virtual brain: 30 years of video-game play and cognitive abilities. Front. Psychol. 4 : 629 . 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00629 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Lemmens J. S., Hendriks S. J. F. (2016). Addictive online games: examining the relationship between game genres and internet gaming disorder. Cyberpsychol. Behav. Soc. Netw. 19 270–276. 10.1089/cyber.2015.0415 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Lemmens J. S., Valkenburg P. M., Peter J. (2011). Psychosocial causes and consequences of pathological gaming. Comput. Hum. Behav. 27 144–152. 10.1016/j.chb.2010.07.015 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Longman H., O’Connor E., Obst P. (2009). The effect of social support derived from World of Warcraft on negative psychological symptoms. Cyberpsychol. Behav. 12 563–566. 10.1089/cpb.2009.0001 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Lucas K., Sherry J. L. (2004). Sex differences in video game play: a communication-based explanation. Commun. Res. 31 499–523. 10.1177/0093650204267930 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Mathiak K. A., Klasen M., Weber R., Ackermann H., Shergill S. S., Mathiak K. (2011). Reward system and temporal pole contributions to affective evaluation during a first person shooter video game. BMC Neurosci. 12 : 66 . 10.1186/1471-2202-12-66 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Mazurek M. O., Engelhardt C. R., Clark K. E. (2015). Video games from the perspective of adults with autism spectrum disorder. Comput. Hum. Behav. 51 122–130. 10.1016/j.chb.2015.04.062 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • McCreery M. P., Krach S. K., Schrader P. G., Boone R. (2012). Defining the virtual self: personality, behavior, and the psychology of embodiment. Comput. Hum. Behav. 28 976–983. 10.1016/j.chb.2011.12.019 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Mehroof M., Griffiths M. D. (2010). Online gaming addiction: the role of sensation seeking, self-control, neuroticism, aggression, state anxiety, and trait anxiety. Cyberpsychol. Behav. 13 313–316. 10.1089/cyber.2009.0229 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Mihara S., Higuchi S. (2017). Cross-sectional and longitudinal epidemiological studies of Internet gaming disorder: a systematic review of the literature. Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 71 425–444. 10.1111/pcn.12532 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Milani L., La Torre G., Fiore M., Grumi S., Gentile D. A., Ferrante M., et al. (2018). Internet gaming addiction in adolescence: risk factors and maladjustment correlates. Int. J. Mental Health Addict. 16 888–904. 10.1007/s11469-017-9750-2 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Möller I., Krahé B. (2009). Exposure to violent video games and aggression in German adolescents: a longitudinal analysis. Aggress. Behav. 35 75–89. 10.1002/ab.20290 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Müller K. W., Beutel M. E., Dreier M., Wölfling K. (2019). A clinical evaluation of the DSM-5 criteria for internet gaming disorder and a pilot study on their applicability to further Internet-related disorders. J. Behav. Addict. 8 16–24. 10.1556/2006.7.2018.140 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Müller K. W., Beutel M. E., Wölfling K. (2014a). A contribution to the clinical characterization of internet addiction in a sample of treatment seekers: validity of assessment, severity of psychopathology and type of co-morbidity. Compr. Psychiatry 55 770–777. 10.1016/j.comppsych.2014.01.010 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Müller K. W., Glaesmer H., Brähler E., Wölfling K., Beutel M. E. (2014b). Prevalence of internet addiction in the general population: results from a German population-based survey. Behav. Inform. Technol. 33 757–766. 10.1080/0144929X.2013.810778 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Müller K. W., Janikian M., Dreier M., Wölfling K., Beutel M. E., Tzavara C., et al. (2015). Regular gaming behavior and internet gaming disorder in European adolescents: results from a cross-national representative survey of prevalence, predictors and psychopathological correlates. Eur. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 24 565–574. 10.1007/s00787-014-0611-2 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Müller K. W., Wölfling K. (2017). Both sides of the story: addiction is not a pastime activity: commentary on: scholars’ open debate paper on the World Health Organization ICD-11 Gaming Disorder proposal (Aarseth et al.). J. Behav. Addict. 6 118–120. 10.1556/2006.6.2017.038 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Nestler S., Back M. D., Egloff B. (2011). Psychometrische Eigenschaften zweier Skalen zur Erfassung interindividueller Unterschiede in der Präferenz zum Alleinsein [Psychometric properties of two scales for the assessment of interindividual differences in preference for solitude]. Diagnostica 57 57–67. 10.1026/0012-1924/a000032 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Newzoo (2017). 2017 Global Games Market Report: Trends, Insights, and Projections Toward 2020. Available at: http://progamedev.net/wp-content/uploads/2017/06/Newzoo_Global_Games_Market_Report_2017_Light.pdf (accessed February 16, 2018). [ Google Scholar ]
  • Ng B. D., Wiemer-Hastings P. (2005). Addiction to the internet and online gaming. Cyberpsychol. Behav. 8 110–113. 10.1089/cpb.2005.8.110 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Nolen-Hoeksema S. (2012). Emotion regulation and psychopathology: the role of gender. Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 8 161–187. 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-032511-143109 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Park J. H., Han D. H., Kim B. N., Cheong J. H., Lee Y. S. (2016). Correlations among social anxiety, self-esteem, impulsivity, and game genre in patients with problematic online game playing. Psychiatry Investig. 13 297–304. 10.4306/pi.2016.13.3.297 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Petry N. M., Rehbein F., Gentile D. A., Lemmens J. S., Rumpf H. J., Mossle T., et al. (2014). An international consensus for assessing internet gaming disorder using the new DSM-5 approach. Addiction 109 1399–1406. 10.1111/add.12457 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Pew Research Center (2018). 5 Facts About Americans and Video Games. Washington, DC: Pew Research Center. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Primack B. A., Carroll M. V., McNamara M., Klem M. L., King B., Rich M. O., et al. (2012). Role of video games in improving health-related outcomes: a systematic review. Am. J. Prevent. Med. 42 630–638. 10.1016/j.amepre.2012.02.023 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Rehbein F., Staudt A., Hanslmaier M., Kliem S. (2016). Video game playing in the general adult population of Germany: can higher gaming time of males be explained by gender specific genre preferences? Comput. Hum. Behav. 55 729–735. 10.1016/j.chb.2015.10.016 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Rosenberg M. (1979). Conceiving the Self. New York, NY: Basic Books. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Rubenstein C., Shaver P. R. (1982). “ The experience of loneliness ,” in Loneliness: A Sourcebook of Current Theory, Research, and Therapy , eds Peplau L. A., Perlman D. (New York, NY: Wiley-Interscience; ), 206–223. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Ryan R. M., Rigby C. S., Przybylski A. (2006). The motivational pull of video games: a self-determination theory approach. Motiv. Emot. 30 344–360. 10.1007/s11031-006-9051-8 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Scharkow M., Festl R., Vogelgesang J., Quandt T. (2015). Beyond the “core-gamer”: genre preferences and gratifications in computer games. Comput. Hum. Behav. 44 293–298. 10.1016/j.chb.2014.11.020 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Schneider L. A., King D. L., Delfabbro P. H. (2018). Maladaptive coping styles in adolescents with Internet gaming disorder symptoms. Int. J. Mental Health Addict. 16 905–916. 10.1007/s11469-017-9756-9 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Schwarzer R., Jerusalem M. (1995). “ Generalized self-efficacy scale ,” in Measures in Health Psychology: A User’s Portfolio. Causal and Control Beliefs , eds Weinman J., Wright S., Johnston M. (Windsor: NFER-NELSON; ), 35–37. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Schwarzer R., Schulz U. (2003). Soziale Unterstützung bei der Krankheitsbewältigung: die Berliner Social Support Skalen (BSSS) [Social support in coping with illness: the Berlin social support scales]. Diagnostica 49 73–82. 10.1026//0012-1924.49.2.73 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Skoric M. M., Teo L. L. C., Neo R. L. (2009). Children and video games: addiction, engagement, and scholastic achievement. Cyberpsychol. Behav. 12 567–572. 10.1089/cpb.2009.0079 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Stopfer J. M., Braun B., Müller K. W., Egloff B. (2015). Narcissus plays video games. Personal. Individ. Differ. 87 212–218. 10.1016/j.paid.2015.08.011 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Taquet P., Romo L., Cottencin O., Ortiz D., Hautekeete M. (2017). Video game addiction: cognitive, emotional, and behavioral determinants for CBT treatment. J. Thér. Comportementale Cogn. 27 118–128. 10.1016/j.jtcc.2017.06.005 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • The Nielsen Company (2017). Games 360 U.S. Report. New York, NY: The Nielsen Company. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Visser M., Antheunis M. L., Schouten A. P. (2013). Online communication and social well-being: how playing World of Warcraft affects players’ social competence and loneliness. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 43 1508–1517. 10.1111/jasp.12144 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • von Collani G., Herzberg P. Y. (2003). Eine revidierte Fassung der deutschsprachigen Skala zum Selbstwertgefühl von Rosenberg [A revised German version of Rosenberg‘s self-esteem scale]. Z. Differ. Diagn. Psychol. 24 3–7. 10.1026/0012-1924/a000032 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Wang H. R., Cho H., Kim D.-J. (2018). Prevalence and correlates of comorbid depression in a nonclinical online sample with DSM-5 internet gaming disorder. J. Affect. Disord. 226 1–5. 10.1016/j.jad.2017.08.005 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Watson D., Clark L. A., Tellegen A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: the PANAS scales. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 54 1063–1070. 10.1037//0022-3514.54.6.1063 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Wölfling K., Beutel M. E., Müller K. W. (2016). “ OSV-S – skala zum onlinesuchtverhalten [AICA-S – Scale for the assessment of internet and computer game addiction] ,” in Diagnostische Verfahren in der Psychotherapie [Diagnostic Measures in Psychotherapy] , eds Geue K., Strauß B., Brähler E. (Göttingen: Hogrefe; ), 362–366. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Wölfling K., Müller K. W., Beutel M. (2011). Reliabilität und Validität der Skala zum Computerspielverhalten [Reliability and validity of the scale for the assessment of pathological computer-gaming]. Psychother. Psychosom. Med. Psychol. 61 216–224. 10.1055/s-0030-1263145 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Wood R. T., Griffith M. D. (2007). A qualitative investigation of problem gambling as an escape-based coping strategy. Psychol. Psychother. 80 107–125. 10.1348/147608306X107881 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Yee N. (2006). Motivations for play in online games. Cyberpsychol. Behav. 9 772–775. 10.1089/cpb.2006.9.772 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]

IMAGES

  1. (PDF) Game Addiction: A Brief Review

    video game addiction research paper

  2. (DOC) CHAPTER 1 (EFFECTS ONLINE GAMES ADDICTION) SUBMITTED BY

    video game addiction research paper

  3. Essay on PUBG Mobile Game Addiction

    video game addiction research paper

  4. (DOC) EFFECTS OF ONLINE GAME ADDICTION TO THE STUDENTS OF

    video game addiction research paper

  5. 🌱 Online games addiction research paper. Gaming Addiction Research

    video game addiction research paper

  6. ⇉Video Game Addiction Essay Essay Example

    video game addiction research paper

VIDEO

  1. GAME ADDICTION Lawsuits Activision, Rockstar, Microsoft, Roblox #gameaddiction #videogameindustry

  2. GOOMERS: The Truth About “Video Game Addiction”

  3. The psychology of gaming addiction

  4. Game Collecting Addiction is NOT what you think it is

  5. Video Game Addiction

  6. Watch me stream WormsZone.io on Omlet Arcade!

COMMENTS

  1. The epidemiology and effects of video game addiction: A systematic

    The present research paper was reported following the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis (PRISMA) guidelines ... This is because they did not report on video game addiction or were of a study design or methodology stated in the exclusion criteria. The remaining 118 articles were then read fully.

  2. Video Game Addiction and Emotional States: Possible Confusion Between

    Video game addiction has been chosen to explore the possible occurrence of this perceptional distortion. A mixed design lab-based study was carried out to compare between video games addicts and non-addicts (between-subjects), and video games-related activities and neutral activities (within-subject). ... A conceptual and methodological ...

  3. Symptoms, Mechanisms, and Treatments of Video Game Addiction

    Introduction and background. Video game addiction falls into the category of Internet gaming disorders (IGDs), which have been strongly correlated with motivational control issues and are regularly compared with gambling [].Many studies have suggested that behavioral addiction can result from compulsive use of the internet [2-4].Although the spectrum of internet addiction includes video gaming ...

  4. (PDF) Game Addiction: A Brief Review

    Paper ID: IJSER171952. 99 of 104 . ... Figure 2: Game Addiction Research Scope . 4. Psychology of Game Addiction. ... Video game addiction is considered as an impulse control .

  5. Internet gaming addiction: current perspectives

    Internet gaming addiction. In recent years, research about Internet gaming addiction has increased both in quantity as well as in quality. Research on gaming addiction dates back to 1983, when the first report emerged suggesting that video gaming addiction is a problem for students. 17 Shortly thereafter, the first empirical study on gaming addiction was published by Shotton, 18 based on self ...

  6. Problematic gaming behaviour and health-related outcomes: A systematic

    SUBMIT PAPER. Journal of Health Psychology. Impact Factor: 3.2 / 5-Year Impact Factor: 3.1 . JOURNAL HOMEPAGE. ... Journal of Addiction Research & Therapy 5(4): e124. Crossref. ... Family-, media-, and school-related risk factors of video game addiction: A 5-year longitudinal study. Journal of Media Psychology 25: 118-128. Crossref. Google ...

  7. (PDF) THE IMPACT OF VIDEO GAME ADDICTION ON SLEEP ...

    the impact of video game addiction on sleep disorder among adolescents and young adults: a systematic review September 2021 International Journal of Information and Decision Sciences 24(S1):1-15

  8. Recent innovations in video game addiction research and theory

    In this paper, we provide an ov erview of recent innov ations in video game addiction research. and highlight the benefit of these contributions to the field. These innovations include: (i ...

  9. Video game addiction: The push to pathologize video games.

    With proposals to include "gaming disorder" in both the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual (DSM) and International Compendium of Diseases (ICD), the concept of video game addiction has gained traction. However, many aspects of this concept remain controversial. At present, little clarity has been achieved regarding diagnostic criteria and appropriate symptoms. It is unclear if symptoms that ...

  10. Online Gaming Addiction and Basic Psychological Needs Among ...

    Technological addictions have become an area of increasing research interest and are conceptualized as non-chemical (i.e., behavioral) addictions (Kuss & Billieux, 2017).Moreover, they can be engaged in actively or passively (Widyanto & Griffiths, 2006).For example, television addiction is a passive technological addiction, whereas smartphone addiction and Internet addiction are active ...

  11. An Investigation into Video Game Addiction in Pre-Adolescents and

    1. Introduction. Most of the free time dedicated to recreational activities is represented by "video games," which respond to a "playful need," captivating people of all ages, sexes, and social classes [].The video game industry is prevalent worldwide, with products running on mobile phones, computers, and video game devices, showing positive effects on basic mental processes such as ...

  12. Frontiers

    1 Center for Mental Health Education, School of Psychology, Southwest University, Chongqing, China; 2 Chongqing Youth and Vocational Technical College, Chongqing, China; As a new type of addictive behaviors and distinct from traditional internet game addiction on desktop computers, mobile game addiction has attracted researchers' attention due to its possible negative effects on mental health ...

  13. The Impact of Online Game Addiction on Adolescent Mental Health: A

    addiction could increase mental health disorders by 1.57 times than adolescents without online game addiction (adjusted odd ratio = 1.57 (1.28-1.94); p ≤ 0.001.

  14. PDF Video Game Addiction: Past, Present and Future

    and video game addiction. This paper begins with a brief past history of how research into video game addiction has changed over the last three decades (i.e., the 1980s, 1990s and 2000s). It then examines more thoroughly the contemporary research literature by analyzing the (i) prevalence of problematic video game use and video game addiction,

  15. Frontiers

    This article is part of the Research Topic Cognitive and Personality Variables in the Development of Behavioral Addictions in Adolescence and Emerging Adulthood View all 10 articles. ... Video game addiction has been chosen to explore the possible occurrence of this perceptional distortion. A mixed design lab-based study was carried out to ...

  16. Frontiers

    Introduction. Video gaming is a very popular leisure activity among adults (Pew Research Center, 2018).The amount of time spent playing video games has increased steadily, from 5.1 h/week in 2011 to 6.5 h/week in 2017 (The Nielsen Company, 2017).Video gaming is known to have some benefits such as improving focus, multitasking, and working memory, but it may also come with costs when it is used ...

  17. Video Game Addiction: Consequences and Treatments

    Abstract: Addiction to video games is a disease recently recognized by the WHO that could increase due to the conditions of confinement caused by the pandemic resulting from COVID-19. This paper investigates the types of addictions, including the consequences of video game addiction in people. Research articles are analyzed, finding that the excessive use of video games is considered a ...

  18. Video gaming addiction and its association with memory, attention and

    Background Examining whether any association exists between addiction to video games and cognitive abilities in children could inform ongoing prevention and management of any possible harm. The objective of this study was to investigate the associations between addiction to video games, and memory, attention and learning abilities among a sample of Lebanese school children. Methods This cross ...

  19. The Association Between Video Gaming and Psychological Functioning

    Introduction. Video gaming is a very popular leisure activity among adults (Pew Research Center, 2018).The amount of time spent playing video games has increased steadily, from 5.1 h/week in 2011 to 6.5 h/week in 2017 (The Nielsen Company, 2017).Video gaming is known to have some benefits such as improving focus, multitasking, and working memory, but it may also come with costs when it is used ...

  20. (PDF) Does video game addiction really exist?

    Abstract. Adolescent video game addiction: issues for the classroom For many, the concept of video game addiction seems far-fetched, particularly if their concepts and defi nitions of addiction ...

  21. (PDF) ONLINE GAMING ADDICTION AND ACADEMIC ATTITUDES ...

    A 2008 press release by the market research group NPD states that: "…of the 174 million gamers who personally play games on PC/Mac or video game systems, three percent are Extreme Gamers ...